SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.66Against words with two main stresses: The case of Guugu Yimidhirr revisitedEffects of morphology in the nativisation of loanwords: The borrowing of /s/ in Xitsonga índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Em processo de indexaçãoCitado por Google
  • Em processo de indexaçãoSimilares em Google

Compartilhar


Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus (SPiL Plus)

versão On-line ISSN 2224-3380
versão impressa ISSN 1726-541X

SPiL plus (Online) vol.66  Stellenbosch  2023

http://dx.doi.org/10.5842/66-1-886 

ARTICLES

 

An Optimality Theoretic analysis of Yoruba hypocoristic personal names: Issues of truncation, reduplication and tone

 

 

Taiwo Oluwaseun Ehineni

Department of African and African American Studies, Harvard University, United States of America E-mail: taiwo_ehineni@fas.harvard.edu

 

 


ABSTRACT

This paper provides an Optimality Theoretic analysis of Yoruba hypocoristic personal names with the aim of showing the interaction of different linguistic processes in the formation of Yoruba names. Based on the data collected from Yoruba texts and interactions in the speech community, this study demonstrates that the formation of the hypocoristics involves not only processes of shortening or reduplication, but also tonal truncation. While Akinlabi and Liberman (2000) note that Yoruba has tonotactic restrictions-where especially vowel-initial words can only take a low or mid tone but not a high tone-, this study reveals that such restriction may be violated in formation of hypocoristics, where reduplicated forms tend rather to satisfy a tonal requirement of HHML to be well-formed. Crucially, the study shows that deriving the hypocoristic in Yoruba involves processes relating not only to the foot structure (foot binarity), where the base of the derived form is expected to be a binary foot, but also, and essentially, processes relating to the tonal structure.

Keywords: Hypocoristic names; reduplication; truncation; tone; Yoruba; onomastics; Optimality Theory


 

 

1. Introduction: hypocoristic names

Hypocoristic names are formed by the truncation, affixation, or reduplication of a particular name to indicate the speaker's affection toward the hearer (Newman & Ahmad 1994). In the literature, hypocoristics are referred to "as pet names, fondling endings, terms of endearment, diminutives, effeminate diminutives, and familiarity markers" (Newman & Ahmad 1994:159). According to van Langendonck (2007), hypocoristics are personal names with a diminutive semantic component. They may be used to mark social status, that is, a way through which a superordinate addresses a subordinate person (Obeng 1997; Seide & Petrulione 2020). In Yoruba, however, hypocoristic names may also be used to show familiarity and intimacy with a peer or younger person (Ola 1995). Furthermore, as Obeng (1997) observes for Akan hypocoristics, there are different contexts of interaction in which Yoruba hypocorsitic names may be used. In Yoruba, as in Akan, these contexts include peer contexts, superior-to-subordinate contexts and subordinate-to-superior interactive contexts. In a superior-to-subordinate context, hypocoristic names express affection, tenderness, the idea of being loved or worth caring for, and may also denote the physical smallness of the referent. This context is commonly seen in parent-child interactions, where the child feels loved and cared for by his/her parent. The peer context refers to the use of hypocoristics among equals, which may be denoted by age or status. In this context, hypocoristic forms indicate solidarity and friendliness.

It is important to note that, among the Yorubas, the use of the hypocoristic varies in terms of age and status. Both are not necessarily the same. For instance, a young person with a high socioeconomic status may not use a hypocoristic form to refer to an older person with a low socioeconomic status. This would be seen as disrespectful and arrogant on the part of the young person. In fact, age is also more significant than professional status in determining the use of a hypocorsitic form. Hence, equality is often a function of age rather than status. Basically, there are social constraints on the use of the hypocoristic among the Yorubas, of which age is the most definitive.

In terms of structure, hypocoristic names may be formed by shortening (Ola 1995) or reduplication (Orie 2002). In some other cases, the tonal pattern may be modified to realize a different tonal pattern in the derived hypocoristic name. For personal names in Yoruba, the following hypocoristic names in (1) are formed through shortening.

The formation of Yoruba hypocoristic names may be analyzed as involving three patterns of shortening which either selects the (/) subject or (ii) the predicate, or (iii) merges both. In (1a) - (1f), the subject or the predicate is selected to be retained/deleted. The first NP is the subject while the second NP is the predicate. For instance, in the name adéolá, the NP adë is the subject while the NP olá is the predicate. The hypocoristic names in (1g)-(1n), however, are formed by shortening by merging the subject and predicate. It is not exactly clear why certain parts are deleted and some are retained. For instance, while olá is retained in the hypocoristic forms in (1a)-(1d), it is not retained in the forms Búkl in and Dámi in (1m). It is also truncated in to Lánre, contrary to (1a) where it occurs similarly in a word initial position and retained. Hence, to adequately account for the diverse patterns in the hypocoristics, it is important to examine the prosodic structure of the hypocoristic forms.

The purpose of the present study is to examine the phonological constraints on hypocoristic formation in Yoruba from an Optimality Theoretic (OT) approach. Hence, in the next section (section 2), a brief introduction to OT as framework is given. Section 3 examines the data which demonstrate the processes of truncation and reduplication in Yoruba hypocoristics, while section 4 presents Optimality Theoretic (OT) analyses of these processes. The results from the analyses indicate that the formation of hypocoristic personal names in Yoruba, either by shortening or reduplication, are based on the prosodic word template or foot. Section 5 closes.

 

2. The Optimality Theory (OT) Framework

Optimality Theory is a formal linguistic model, in which grammars consist of a universal set of violable constraints that are ranked in a language-particular hierarchy. Lower-ranked constraints are often forcibly violated in order to improve satisfaction of higher-ranked constraints. The optimal or most harmonic pronunciation of a given word is that output candidate which best fulfills the language-specific ranking for a selected input form (See Prince and Smolensky 2004, McCarthy 2007). Crucially, OT is a theory of constraint interaction in grammar. An OT grammar has three components. CON is the component that defines the set of universal violable constraints. GEN is the component where output candidate parses are generated based on input forms. EVAL is the component that selects an optimal output from the set of alternative candidates, given a language-specific hierarchical ordering of Con. The path from the input to the output is specified below:

As McCarthy (2007:4) explains OT sets up a basic dichotomy between the operational component of the grammar and the constraint component. The operational component, called GEN, constructs a set of candidate output forms that deviate from the input in various ways. The constraint component, called EVAL, selects a member of this set to be the actual output of the grammar. In other words, a set of candidates is obtained from GEN, evaluated by EVAL using a constraint hierarchy, and the candidate that violates top-ranked constraints less frequently is chosen as the grammar's output.

Constraints may be divided into major categories: markedness and faithfulness. Markedness constraints relates to linguistic well-formedness (i.e complex consonant clusters); however, faithfulness constraints are inherently conservative, ensuring that output resemble its input. Because markedness constraints favour some linguistic structures over others, they are often in tension with faithfulness constraints, which resist changes to input structures. This tension is called constraint conflict, and it is resolved in OT by ranking.

Hence, this study would identify crucial constraints in Yoruba grammar and how these interact in the realization of optimal forms in the formation of hypocoristic names.

 

3. The structure of Yoruba Hypocoristic Forms

Yoruba hypocoristic names show basic processes of truncation and reduplication. In fact, a major area where truncation occurs in Yoruba is the area of hypocoristic name formation. According to Ola (1995), names in Yoruba may be regularly shortened to either VCV or CVCV - a binary (bimoraic or disyllabic) foot. Furthermore, as Ola (1995:195) observes, the shortened names always correspond to the leftmost segmental materials of the morpheme. For instance, a name such as kóláwolé may be realized as hold or wolé, while a name like foldmi may only be shortened as fold - other forms such as *lami or *mi are impossible. The latter form *mi is ruled out by the foot-based requirement, and although the former form *lami is not ruled out by this requirement, it may not be an optimal shortened form. Ola (1995) therefore proposes that truncation in Yoruba is also constrained by a leftmostness requirement, and it is this requirement that rules out *lami from being optimal. This leftmostness requirement is also what is responsible for the possibility of variants in truncated forms. Hence, Ola (1995) suggests some prosodic and leftmostness requirements for a truncation analysis which are presented below in (2).

These constraints are used to analyze the truncation process in names. According to Ola (1995:160), the truncation of the name folámí may be represented as follows in the tableau in (3).

Ola (1995) explains that, even though candidate (3a) satisfies Trunc=Foot, it is still sub-optimal because it violates Align Trunc and Anchor. This candidate lámí (3a) violates Align Trunc since its left edge does not align with the left edge of the morpheme in the base (folámí). Also, while Anchor requires corespondence at the initial and final segments of the base (right and left edges), candidate (3a) lámí only corresponds with the final segment from the base (folá-mi). Hence, it is not anchored at the initial segment, and therefore fails to satisfy Anchor. Candidate (3b) - a monosyllabic word - is ruled out by violating Trunc=Foot, while candidate (3c) fails by violating Cont; the truncated form in (3c) does not constitute a contiguous string from the base. Hence, the last candidate (3d) wins by not violating any higher-ranked constraints and is not fatally penalized for violating Parse, which is lower-ranked. It violates Parse because the final syllable [mi] of the base is not realized in the truncated form.

There is, however, another major candidate which is not mentioned by Ola (1995), namely olá. As discussed in the following section, olá may also be ruled out by the constraint ranking suggested in the tableau in (3), since it does not satisfy Align Trunc and Anchoring constraints, thus making the candidate (3d) the only optimal form. As shown in the tableau in (3'), the fifth candidate olá (3'e) violates Align Trunc since the [f] at the left edge of the full name is not at the left edge of the truncated name. Similarly, it violates Anchoring since the form olá does not correspond to the initial segment of the base (though it agrees with the final segment of the base - folá-mi).

Another process that has been discussed in Yoruba hypocoristic forms is duplication, also known as reduplication (see Ola 1995; Abiodun 1997; Adewole 1997). Pulleyblank (2009) identifies four major types of reduplicative patterns in Yoruba and discusses how each exhibits rather different properties. Pulleyblank (2009:311) explains that, on one hand, agentive reduplication (4i) involves two identical copies of a base, with both segmental and tonal material copied, while infixing reduplication (4ii) involves a complete copy of the base supplemented by a segmentally and tonally fixed infix. Also, whereas distributive reduplication (4iii) copies part of a base, restricting its copying to segmental material, gerundive reduplication (4iv) copies only part of a base, mixing the copied material with lexically specified material (Pulleyblank 2009:311). He further claims that these four reduplicative patterns involve complete or partial identity between a base and a reduplicant resulting from morphological copying or the satisfaction of prosodic requirements. These reduplicative patterns are presented below in (4).

According to Pulleyblank (2009), these reduplicative patterns exhibit different unique properties. The first type of reduplication (agentive reduplication, 4i) involves the faithful reduplication of a verb base combination - a form of total reduplication requiring correspondence between the base and the reduplicant. The second class of reduplication (kí infixation, 4ii) deals with the use of word-linker between two identical noun components. Pulleyblank (2009) notes that the presence of is due to the morphology, not to the phonology. Morphologically, (plus reduplication) indicates the meaning of "any" or "whichever". However, just like the agentive reduplication, the kí-forms also involve total reduplication through exact copying of the base in conjunction with an infixed morphological linker. Contrary to the initial two reduplicative types, the distributive case (4iii) is a foot-based pattern, involving the copy of an initial VCV sequence.

As Pulleyblank (2009) explains, the distributive type of reduplication involves the copy of a single foot. If the base constitutes a single binary foot, then the entire base is copied (i.e, ewé 'leaf - eweewé 'every leaf). In contrast, if the base is longer than a binary foot, then only the initial VCV sequence is copied (i.e ojoojúmç 'every day'). The last case of reduplication (gerundive reduplication, 4iv) is derived by affixing a [Cí] prefix, where the "C" is a copy of the first consonant of the verbal base. Thus, the formation of the gerundive is an example of partial reduplication, just like the distributive case.

Basically, Pulleyblank's (2009) study reveals that reduplication in Yoruba can be either partial or total, and may be influenced by phonological or morphological requirements. For instance, distributive reduplication satisfies a phonological requirement (only a foot is copied), but in the kí-form, reduplication is motivated by morphology - the infixation of kí. In fact, the entire base (not a prosodic constituent) is reduplicated. Thus, we can say that partial reduplication often seem to be clearly phonologically motivated (a prosodic constituent may be copied), while total reduplication provides the domain for morphological motivation.

Another important aspect of reduplication that Pulleyblank (2009) considers is the issue of tone. He claims:

Reduplication in Yoruba exhibits 'tonal neutralization' - where if the first mora of the base is M, then both vowels of the distributive are M, but if the first mora of the base is L, then both vowels of the distributive are L.

(Pulleyblank 2009:324-325)

This notion of tone neutralization is formalized by Pulleyblank as a rule of leftward spreading in Yoruba reduplication, where the tone in the base spreads leftwards in the reduplicant (as exemplified in the distributive). Thus, Pulleyblank (2009:337) comments:

Many languages systematically require tonal specifications to be left-aligned - the left-edge asymmetry is built into early autosegmental treatments of tone such as Leben (1973) and Goldsmith (1976), we see left-edge preference emerging under reduplication.

The present study draws heavily from Pulleyblank's (2009) discussion of reduplication in Yoruba - especially the fact that reduplication may be phonologically induced in certain contexts in the language.

More significantly, however, is the fact that the present study makes certain observations on reduplication not covered in Pulleyblank's (2009) study, the first of which is that phonologically-induced reduplication occurs in Yoruba hypocoristic names. Second, the leftward spreading rule identified by Pulleyblank (2009) as occuring in Yoruba reduplication does not apply to reduplication in Yoruba hypocoristics. In fact, the tonal constituency of the hypocoristics functions very differently from the prosodic constituency of the hypocoristics. In other words, while reduplication in hypocoristics is prosodically motivated, the tone is not. Third, it has been widely claimed about tone behavior in Yoruba "that H tones may not occur on a word-initial vowel" (Pulleyblank 2009:341, see also Ola 1995; Akinlabi & Liberman 2000, 2001). However, in hypocoristics, a word-initial vowel may take high tones. This would consequently lead to the violation of Ola's (1995) influential tone constraint *[V to prohibit a bare High-toned initial V from occurring at the beginning of a word or phrase. Conceptually, this violation of a relevant faithfulness constraint of *[V in the formation of hypocoristics exhibits a pattern of the "emergence of the unmarked" (McCarthy & Prince 1994). These issues will be explored in detail in the next section, focusing specifically on reduplication in Yoruba hypocoristic personal names.

 

4. Analysis Of Yoruba Hypocoristic Personal Names 4.1 The Process of Truncation

The present analysis of shortened/truncated hypocoristic forms will be formalized based on theoretical discussions of output-output correspondence (see Kager 2004) and the Yoruba-related constraints in shortening processes provided by Ola (1995). However, based on the framework of output-output correspondence in morphological truncation, a model of correspondence relations in Yoruba hypocoristics is presented below in (5).

The I (Input) refers to the level of lexical entries as exemplified in (5j) by temi 'mine', ni 'is' and oluwa 'God' where the B (Base) is the stem that is non-truncated. It is from this base that truncation occurs to realize T - the truncated form. A correspondence relation holds between the base, its input and the truncated form, since forms like I-temi.ni.oluwa, B-temiloluwa and T- temi share some featural similarities. However, certain constraints specifically trigger the occurrence of the truncated forms (T) in Yoruba. Some of these constraints include the constraints provided by Kager (2004) and Ola (1995) which were discussed in previous sections.

First, from the data in (1), reproduced below in (1'), it is observed that the realized truncated form is a bisyllabic structure. For instance, the hypocoristic forms in (1a-d) are V.CV, while all other forms have CV.CV structure.

It is suggested that the hypocoristic names all reflect a binary foot (bisyllabic) in prosodic terms. This therefore motivates the application of Ft Bin (foot binarity), where the truncated form is a foot (Trunc = Foot), and BT (base-truncated form) refers to identity constraints. These prosodic and identity constraints are explained in (6).

The constraints in (6) are necessary because Yoruba hypocoristic forms generally involve a process of structural reduction, but the reduced forms may not be smaller than a binary foot.

Thus, it is expected that there would be a constraint hierarchy where Foot Binarity (Ft Bin) is ranked higher than the faithfulness constraint Max-BT to be able to satisfy the minimal requirement of a bisyllabic foot in the output. This analysis of constraint ranking is provided in the tableau in (7).

In the tableau in (7), candidate (7b) olúwa is ruled out since it fatally violates all constraints -it violates Max-BT through deletion of initial segments and violates Trunc = Foot since it is not a bisyllabic foot. On the other hand, faithful candidate (7a) tèmilolúwa satisfies Max-BT since nothing is deleted but fails to satisfy a higher ranked constraint, Trunc = Foot, since it is more than a bisyllabic foot. The optimal candidate is témï (7c), which, though it violates Max-BT, satisfies the higher ranked Trunc = Foot. Crucially, the ranking of Trunc = Foot over Max-BT produces the winning candidate in the hypocoristic form. However, these two constraints alone may not rule out other eligible candidates. For instance, lolú is a possible candidate but not an optimal hypocoristic form for the name Tèmilolúwa. To account for the disqualification of lolú, another constraint must be considered.

it is generally observed that the hypocoristic forms always comform to the leftmost segmental materials of the morpheme. For instance, a name such as akinolá may be reduced to either akin or olá, whereas a name like foláké may only be shortened to folá, forms such as láké or are disallowed. The latter form is ruled out by the foot binarity restriction proposed earlier. The question is, however, why the binary footed láké is not allowed in the derived form. Moreover, why does the name olúwaségun have two hypocoristic variants - olú or ségunl To answer these quesions, I suggest that hypocorization targets the leftmost properties of the morpheme. Hence, aside from the templatic requirement of foot binarity, there is a leftmostness constraint which is formalized as follows in (8), based on Ola (1995).

The constraint in 8 is given to account for the fact that the left edge of a hypocoristic form is observed to be identical with the left edge of a morpheme in the original name. This constraint would, for instance, disqualify lolú as a valid hypocoristic derivation from Tèmilolúwa. in addition, the constraint in (8) may shed light on the possibility of having two hypocoristic variants. The Align Trunc constraint will be used with previously identified constraints Trunc=Foot and Max-BT to produce the optimal candidate. The analysis of the disqualification of lolú is presented in the tableau in (9) below.

Essentially, candidate (9d) lolú is ruled out by Align Trunc since it does not align with the left edge of the morpheme (i.e tèmi or olúwa). Although both candidates (9a) and (9b) do not violate Align Trunc, they are ruled out by Trunc=Foot, as previously explained. However, note that there is no ranking argument in the constraint hierarchy. in other words, if the ranking of constraints is reversed, candidate (9c) would still be the winning candidate. A fuller analysis is provided in (10), including a variety of other possible candidates for the hypocoristic form of the name Tèmilolúwa.

The new candidates lúwa (10c) and wa (10d) all violate high ranked constraints Align Trunc and Max-BT, while mi (10f) violates Trunc = Foot and Max-BT. Both candidates lúwa and wa are ruled out by Align Trunc since they do not correspond to the left edge of the morphemes in the base. Candidate (10f) mi is monosyllabic and therefore violates Trunc = Foot. Thus, all the new candidates are "harmonically bounded" since they cannot win regardless of the ranking of constraints. crucially, thus, the truncation of forms is influenced by the prosodic requirement of foot binarity.

4.2 The Process of Reduplication

in this section, specific examples of Yoruba hypocoristic personal names formed through reduplication are analyzed following perspectives discussed in sections 2 and 3.1. Yoruba hypocoristic personal names formed through reduplication are presented in (11).

In forming a hypocoristic through reduplication, certain features are observed. First, the first foot of the original name may be reduplicated as shown in cases such as (11a) akinolá -akinakin, (11b) iféolú - íféifè, (11c) òjóadé - ójóojò and so on. Second, the other foot in the name may also be reduplicated as manifested in alternative forms like (11a) akinolá - çláolà, (11b) ïféolú - ólúolü, (11c) òjóadé - ádéadè. Third, the tonal structure of the original form may not be preserved in the derived form. Foot structure and tonal structure are distinctive aspects of the reduplicated hypocoristic. While a foot from the original name is reduplicated, the tone from the original name is not. It is significant to point out that the new tonal structure in the reduplicated hypocoristic distinguishes the hypocoristic as a unique form separate from the original name. In other words, while the reduplicant is derived from the base and has similar foot structure with the base, the tone designates the reduplicant as a unique form. For instance, (11a) çláolà is derived from the foot olá in [akinolá] but has a different tonal structure (HH.ML) from the foot (MH) which will be further discussed in section 3.3. In evaluating the reduplicated hypocoristic forms from the OT approach, the following constraints are considered.

Note that the constraint Red = Foot is modeled after the truncation constraint Trunc=Foot utilized in our discussion of truncation. Both constraints (Red = Foot and Trunc=Foot) relate to the general idea of foot binarity - Ft Bin. This constraint is a major prosodic requirement that governs both truncation and reduplication in the hypocoristics, unlike other constraints such as Max-BT which applies specifically to truncation and Max-BR that applies specifically to reduplication. The constraint Red = Foot requires that a reduplicated form in the hypocoristic be a foot. This constraint imposes a prosodic requirement on the output form and is an undominated constraint in evaluating reduplicated hypocoristics. This analysis of the constraint interaction is presented in the tableaux in (13) and (14).

In tableaux (13) and (14), the candidate (13a, 14a) akinolá.akinolá is ruled out by Red = Foot since the reduplicant (RED) is more than a foot. However, (13b) akin.akin and (14b) álá.olà are optimal forms by having a reduplicant that is a foot. Though (13b) akin.akin and (14b) álá.olà violate Max-IO, since segments in the input are deleted in the output, they still win because Max-IO is low ranked. Crucially, these indicate that Red = Foot need to outrank Max-IO in the constraint hierarchy to produce an optimal reduplicated form. To further buttress the ranking of Red = Foot above Max-IO, analysis of hypocoristic forms of names such as òjóadé and adésínà are presented in tablueax (15) and (16).

The candidates (15a) òjóadé.òjóadé and (16a) adésínà.adésínà are faithful by ensuring that all the segments in the input are in the output form. These candidates are, however, ruled out by violating Red = Foot, since their reduplicated parts - [òjóadé] in òjóadé.òjóadé and [adésínà] in adésínà.adésínà are more than a foot. On other hand, the other candidates (15c, 16b,c) win by satisfying Red = Foot, since their reduplicated parts constitute a foot (i.e òjó.òjó and ádé.adè). This further reinforces that Red = Foot is an undominated constraint in deriving the optimal hypocoristic forms of the names. However, the constraints already identified (Red = Foot, Max-IO) may not be able to rule out possible but not optimal candidates, like akin.akinolá. For instance, see the tableau in (17).

Both candidates (17b) and (17c) satisfy the high ranked Red = Foot constraint, but candidate (17b) violates Max-IO and candidate (17c) does not. This therefore suggests candidate (17c) ákín.akinolá is the winning candidate. However, (17c) ákín.akinolá is not an optimal hypocoristic in Yoruba. Thus, a base-reduplicant identity constraint needs to be examined to rule out ákín.akinolá. Note that Max-IO does not contend against the deletion of input segments in the reduplicant. This is because "the reduplicant has no input segments of its own, to which it might potentially be faithful" (Kager 2004:207). This constraint is presented below in (18).

This constraint ensures that there is correspondence between the base and the reduplicant. In other words, it militates against deletion of segments in the reduplicant. Thus, while Max-IO may not be able to rule out ákín.akinolá, Max-BR would rule it out, as shown in the tableau in (19).

In the tableau in (19), candidate (19c) ákín.akinolá fails to satisfy Max-BR. In the form [ákín.akinolá], the base -akinolá does not correspond with the reduplicant -ákín. But in candidate (19b), the base -ákín corresponds with the reduplicant -akin. Note that they both satisfy Red = Foot since the reduplicant in both candidates (19b,c) conform to a foot. However, (19b) ákín.akin wins by satisfying higher ranked Max-BR which ákín.akinolá fails to satisfy. Crucially, Max-BR needs to be ranked higher than Max-IO to produce the winning candidate. Hence, the ranking of the reduplicative identity constraints over input-output faithfulness constraints is what makes the reduplicant more identical to the base than the input segments in reduplication.

Additionally, as suggested by examples (11g-l), some hypocoristic forms do not have variants. The name (11h) mosúnmolá, for instance, may only be reduplicated as mósúnmosün, not mç>lámç>là. Also, while olá.olà is a possible hypocoristic form (see the tableau in 15), it is not an optimal form for the name mosúnmolá - even though it satisfies the undominated constraint Red = Foot. Therefore, we will examine why ç>lá.ç>là is not a possible hypocoristic form in mosúnmolá. Also, we will discuss why the names in (11g-l) may have only one hypocoristic form, unlike the names in (11a-f) that have more than one hypocoristic form. First it should be noted that these names in (11g-l) are lexicalized sentential names and are therefore more morphologically complex compared to names in (11a-f). Furthermore, it is observed that the hypocoristic forms of the names in (11g-l) tend to favor the leftmost edge of the name. Hence, another constraint is suggested to analyze these other forms: Linearity (McCarthy & Prince 1995). This constraint and another alignnment constraint- namely, Align Red -are formalized more clearly in (20).

Using the constraints in (20), the analysis of the derivation of the hypocoristic for mosúnmçlá is presented in tableau (21).

It should be noted that the constraint Linearity is different from Align Red in the sense that, while the latter aligns the reduplicant to the left edge of the morpheme, the former requires that the reduplicant conform to the linear order in the input. Thus, in tableau (21), candidate (21c) mçlá.mçlá does not violate Align Red since its left edge aligns to the left edge of a morpheme (i.e mó). However, it violates Linearity since the entire word mólá.mólá does not preserve the linear order - [mó-olá] - in the input. This is because the second back vowel in the input [mó -olá] is deleted in the derived output [mó-lá], which deviates from the input linear order [mó-olà], showing a sequence of vowels at mopheme boundary. While the candidate ósún.osün does not violate Linearity, it fatally violates Align Red since its left edge deviates from the left edge of a morpheme in the input. Candidate (21e) çlá.çlá satisfies all high ranked constraints (just like the winning candidate - [21b] mósún.mosün), but incurs more violations of Max-IO. The winning candidate is (21b) mósún.mosün since it is the most harmonic with the constraint hierarchy.

Apart from the processes of truncation and reduplication, a major factor in the formation of the hypocoristic is tone. The derived hypocoristic is characterized by a unique tonal structure, which is discussed in section 3.3.

4.3 The Process of Tonal Derivation

Previous sections have specifically focused on how hypocoristic personal names in Yoruba are formed through shortening and reduplication. However, tone also functions uniquely as a separate feature of the hypocoristic, especially in the reduplicated forms. In the data given in (1), the tonal structure of the derived hypocoristic does not conform with the base even though the output is derived from the base. Thus, while the reduplicant is prosodically derived from the base, its tone does not clearly reflect the base. Second, the derived tonal patterns appears to have a specific structure - H.H.M.L (High.High.Mid.Low). That is, regardless of the tone on the original name, the hypocoristic form takes a H.H.M.L tonal structure. This is significant to underscore because this is contrary to Akinlabi and Liberman's (2000:8) assertion that Yoruba has tonotactic restrictions where vowel-initial words especially can only take a low or mid tone, but not a high tone. However, in hypocoristic forms of personal names, high tones may occur vowel-initially as previously shown by the data in (1). For clarity, these vowel-initial names where high tone occur are reproduced below in (22).

Note that the derived tonal pattern is not in any way motivated by the tonal structure of the original name. That is, the entire reduplicated hypocoristic, not just the reduplicant, always has a specific tonal form which is distinct from the base form. The realization of tonal truncation in the hypocoristic is influenced by other constraints not previously discussed. These constraints are summarized below in (23).

It is important to reiterate that the Align Red Hypo-T constraint specifically ensures that the reduplicated hypocoristic is realized with a High-High-Mid-Low tone in the optimal form. This tonal requirement does not hold for truncated hypocoristics. The constraint *[V (Ola 1995) prohibits a high-toned initial vowel from occurring at the beginning of a word or phrase and has been further identified as a major tone requirement in Yoruba (Akinlabi & Liberman 2000). Max-T is a faithfulness constraint that militates any deletion of tone in the output form. The analysis of how these constraints interact in deriving the tonally optimal candidate in the hypocoristic is presented in the following tablueax in (24) and (25).

In tableau (24), candidate (24a) is a tonally faithful candidate by maintaining the tonal pattern from the original name, and so does not violate *[V and Max-T. It is, however, disqualified by Align Red Hypo-T, since the hypocoristic tone pattern is not realized. Candidate (24b) violates Max-T since an input tone is deleted in the output but is principally disqualified by Align Red Hypo-T since it lacks the required tonal pattern (H.H.M.L) in the derived hypocoristic. The third candidate (24c) is optimal since it satisfies the undominated constraint Align Red Hypo-T by having the required tonal pattern H.H.M.L even though it violates Max-T. Other tonally possible but non-optimal candidates are considered in tableau (25).

As in tableau (24), the nonoptimal candidates (25a-d) are ruled out by violating the constraint Align Red Hypo-T. Thus, candidate (25e) is the winner since it does not violate this high-ranked constraint in the derived hypocoristic. Both tableaux in (24) and (25) show that there is a critical ranking of constraints in producing the optimal candidate. That is, if the constraint ranking is flipped the other way, the tonally faithful candidate (25a) semísémi, which is not the tonally optimal hypocoristic, will be considered the winner. The faithful candidate incurs fewer violations of constraints but is disqualified by not satisfying a higher-ranked tonal constraint in the hypocoristic.

of more importance is the fact that the derived hypocoristic may violate expected or regular tonal patterns in Yoruba. As previously noted, in Yoruba, there is a high-tone restriction in vowel-initial words in Yoruba (Akinlabi & Liberman 2000, 2001). However, this constraint is violable when deriving hypocoristic names. This can be seen in the following tableau in (26).

Note that candidates (26a) and (26b) do not violate *[V since they are vowel-initial words starting with a low (L) and Mid (M) tone respectively. The third candidate (26c), on the other hand, starts with a high tone which is disallowed in vowel-initial words in the language. However, candidates (26a,b) are rejected by Align Red Hypo-T - a higher ranked constraint in the reduplicated hypocoristic. Candidate (26c) is optimal since it fulfills the tonotactic requirement of the reduplicated hypocoristic H.H.M.L.

Essentially, in deriving optimal hypocoristic names in Yoruba, there is a significant interaction between the process of reduplication, prosodic categories (foot) and tone. While a hypocoristic name may be formed by reduplication, high ranked constraints such as Red = Foot and Align Red Hypo-T impose prosodic and tonal constraints on the output form.

Furthermore, tone in reduplicated hypocoristic personal names behaves differently from other forms of reduplication discussed in Yoruba. on the nature of tone in Yoruba distributive reduplication, Pulleyblank (2009: 324) comments that "the tone is predictable from the moras of the reduplicant, where both moras of the reduplicant are M [Mid tone] if the first mora of the base is M and L [Low tone] and that tones may not occur on a word-initial vowel". Pulleyblank's (2009) view is anchored on cited examples such as ewé - eweewé, odún -odoodún, osü - osoosü, where the tone of the reduplicant is derived from the initial mora in the base. However, in Yoruba hypocoristic reduplication, not discussed by Pulleyblank (2009) in the various Yoruba reduplicative patterns, the tonal structure of the reduplicant is not mora-predictable. A specific tonotactic structure is required in the output different from the prosodic requirements. That is, regardless of the moraic structure of the base, the tone on the reduplicant in the hypocoristic is expected to be H.H.M.L. That is, the reduplicated hypocoristic construction has its own tone melody that supercedes that of the base.

Consequently, the "rule of leftward spreading" (Pulleyblank 2009:334), postulated to account for tone movement in Yoruba reduplication, may not necessarily apply in the hypocoristic. That is, while the M tone moves leftward in Yoruba reduplicated forms such as ewé - eweewé, odún - odoodún, osü - osoosü (provided by Pulleyblank), it does not do so in reduplicated hypocoristics such as akinolá - ákínakm, wúràolá - oláolà. In fact, the tone in the reduplicated foot does not reflect the tone of the original foot in the base, since the tone melody is a property of the reduplicated hypocoristic construction.

 

5. Conclusion

This study has examined the role of foot structure and binarity in the formation of hypocoristic personal names in Yoruba. The foot, which is the constituent under which moras and syllables are grouped, serve as the basis for deriving the hypocoristic names. Furthermore, the notion of binarity in a foot requires the grouping of moras or syllables in twos. That is, when a hypocoristic name is formed, it is reduced to a bisyllabic or bimoraic foot. Forms that violate this structural requirement are disqualified. Hence, the well-formedness of a foot is influenced by the principle of binarity; a foot is maximally binary either at the moraic or the syllabic level (Prince 1991; McCarthy & Prince 1993). Therefore, the process of deriving hypocoristic forms of personal names in Yoruba is a foot-dependent prosodic process which maps sufficient segmental material from the base to foot in order to satisfy the templatic requirement.

Furthermore, foot structure and tonal structure are distinctive aspects of the reduplicated hypocoristic. This is due to the fact that, when deriving reduplicated hypocoristic forms, the reduplicant does not always retain the tonality of its original base, but retains a foot structure from the base. The discussion of reduplicated hypocoristics reveals that foot faithfulness should be distinguished from tonal faithfulness in reduplication. Hence, the idea of morphological identity between base and reduplicant (Downing 1997a,b,c) does not necessarily imply that morphology will assign the same tonal patterns to the two copies involved in reduplication.

In sum, the formation of hypocoristic personal names in Yoruba, either by shortening or reduplication, are based on the prosodic word template or foot. This reinforces the idea that morphological derivations may be determined by prosodic requirements. Put differently, the prosodic word is prefered to the lexical word. For instance, olúwa is a lexical word but is not a binary foot - not a bisyllabic or bimoraic foot, it is therefore not an optimal hypocoristic form. Finally, the formation of hypocoristic personal names may deviate from regular tonal patterns in Yoruba by requiring that an optimal form be H.H.M.L. This leads to the generally high-ranked tonal constraint *[V in Yoruba to be dominated in the hypocoristic.

 

References

Abíódún, M.A. 1997. More on reduplication in Yorùbá. Journal of Nigerian Languages and Literatures, 5: 93-98.         [ Links ]

Adewole, S.M. 1997. Reduplication in Yoruba. Linguistique Africaine, 18: 105-129.         [ Links ]

Akinlabi, A. & M. Liberman. 2000. The tonal phonology of Yoruba clitics. In B. Gerlach & J. Grijzenhoul (eds.), Clitics in Phonology, Morphology and Syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 31-62. https://doi.org/10.1075/la.36.04aki        [ Links ]

Akinlabi, A. & M. Liberman. 2001. Tonal complexes and alignment. In M. Kim& U. Strauss (eds.), Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society, 31: 1-20. https://doi.org/10.1075/la.36.04aki

Downing, L.J. 1997a. Correspondence effects in Siswati reduplication. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences, 25: 81-95.         [ Links ]

Downing, L.J. 1997b. Morphological correspondence in Kinande Reduplication. Proceedings of Berkeley Linguistics Society, 23: 83-94. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v23i1.1277        [ Links ]

Downing, L.J. 1997c. Morphological correspondence on Kikerewe Reduplication. Proceedings of WCCFL, 16: 161-174.         [ Links ]

Ehineni, T.O. 2017. Prosodic reduplication in Yoruba. Journal of Universal Language, 18(2): 39-59. https://doi.org/10.22425/jul.2017.18.2.39        [ Links ]

Goldsmith, J. 1976. Autosegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, MIT.         [ Links ]

Kager, R. 2004. Optimality Theory. London: Cambridge University Press.         [ Links ]

Leben, W. 1973. Suprasegmental phonology. Doctoral dissertation, MIT. New York, Garland, 1980.         [ Links ]

McCarthy, J. & A. Prince. 1993. Generalized alignment. In G. Booij & J. van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology. Dordrecht: Springer. pp. 79-153. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3712-8 4        [ Links ]

McCarthy, J. & A. Prince. 1994. Two lectures on prosodic morphology. Handouts of two lectures at OTS/HIL Workshop on Prosodic Morphology, Utrecht University, July 1994. [ROA- 59, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html]

McCarthy, J. & A. Prince. 1995. Prosodic morphology. In J. Goldsmith (ed.), The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Blackwell. pp. 318-366.

McCarthy, J. 1995. Extensions of faithfulness: Rotuman revisited. Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series, 36. MA: University of Massachusetts. [ROA-110, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html]        [ Links ]

McCarthy, J.J. 2007. What is Optimality Theory? Language and Linguistics Compass, 93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00018.x        [ Links ]

Newman, P. & A. Mustapha. 1994. Hypocoristics in Hausa. Anthropological Linguistics, 34: 59-172.         [ Links ]

Obeng, G.S. 1997. From morphophonology to sociolinguistics: The case of Akan hypocoristic day-names. Multilingua, 16(1): 40-56. https://doi.org/10.1515/mult.1997.16.1.39        [ Links ]

Ola, O. 1995. Optimality in Benue-Congo prosodic phonology and morphology. PhD. Dissertation, University of British Columbia.         [ Links ]

Orie, O. 2002. Yoruba names and gender marking. Anthropological Linguistics, 44(2): 115-142.         [ Links ]

Prince, Α. 1991. Quantitative consequences of rhythmic organization. Chicago Linguistic Society (Parasession on the syllable in phonetics and phonology) 26.2.355-98. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470759400

Prince, Α. & P. Smolensky. 2004. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Malden, MA, and Oxford, UK: Blackwell. [Revision of 1993 technical report, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. Available on Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-537]

Pulleyblank, D. 2009. Patterns of reduplication in Yoruba. In K. Hanson & S. Inkelas (eds.), The nature of the word: Studies in honor of Paul Kiparsky. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262083799.003.0014        [ Links ]

Seide, M.S. & P. Petrulione. 2020. Formation and usage of hypocoristics form in Lithuanian and Brazilian Portuguese. Alfa: Revista de Lingüística, 64: 1-25.         [ Links ]

Van Langendonck, W. 2007. Theory and typology of proper names. Berlin: Walter de Gruyer. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197853        [ Links ]

 

 

1 Note that phonetic representation of the names are also provided after the names. This is to clarify the status of /n/ in Yoruba hypocoristics. In the names given (specifically (11a) akinolá, (11f) olúwaségun, (11h) mosúnmolá, ipíndolà) /n/ occurs after a vowel to indicate a nasal vowel. In other words, when /n/ occurs in a syllable final position, it often indicates that the immediate preceding vowel is a nasal vowel. This point is necessary since /n/ may also be a syllabic nasal in a context like nlánlá in Yoruba, where it may be moraic. (see Ehineni 2017 for more discussion).

Creative Commons License Todo o conteúdo deste periódico, exceto onde está identificado, está licenciado sob uma Licença Creative Commons