SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.52 índice de autoresíndice de assuntospesquisa de artigos
Home Pagelista alfabética de periódicos  

Serviços Personalizados

Artigo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • Em processo de indexaçãoCitado por Google
  • Em processo de indexaçãoSimilares em Google

Compartilhar


Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus (SPiL Plus)

versão On-line ISSN 2224-3380
versão impressa ISSN 1726-541X

SPiL plus (Online) vol.52  Stellenbosch  2017

http://dx.doi.org/10.5842/52-0-712 

ARTICLES

 

Distribution of conjunctive and disjunctive forms in Xitsonga*

 

 

Seunghun J. LeeI; Crous HlungwaniI, II

IInternational Christian University, Japan. E-mail: seunghunjlee@gmail.com
IIUniversity of Venda, South Africa. E-mail: crous.hlungwane@univen.ac.za

 

 


ABSTRACT

The conjunctive and disjunctive forms in Xitsonga are examined with the purpose of presenting the distribution of these forms. While verbs in the conjunctive form are followed by some elements, the disjunctive is used when no element follows the verb. Xitsonga follows these basic patterns observed in other Bantu languages, but previous theories cannot explain all of the cases in which the conjunctive and disjunctive forms are distinguished. In previous work, three major approaches have been proposed: the constituency approach, the focus-based approach, and the information packaging approach. Xitsonga supports but also provides counterevidence to all of these approaches. This paper also re-examines the claim that the presence of conjunctive/disjunctive distinctions only exists in the present tense. Following Creissels (2014), we report that the conjunctive/disjunctive dichotomy is present in other tenses as well when prosodic patterns such as penultimate lengthening are further examined.

Keywords: Xitsonga, conjunctive, disjunctive, information packaging theory


 

 

1. Introduction

The goal of this paper is to present a comprehensive picture of disjunctive and conjunctive forms in Xitsonga main clauses, which are an under-investigated phenomenon in Xitsonga. The paper will also report phonological patterns that distinguish these forms that are otherwise morphologically identical.

The disjunctive and conjunctive forms have been referred to by various names, as in (1). In descriptive work on Bantu languages, the forms are called "long form" and "short form", based on how the morpheme is realised. In Sharman (1956), whose work is concerned with prosody, the two forms were called "weak link" and "strong link", respectively. With reference to information structural status, Givón distinguishes the forms in terms of types of focus.

(1) Disjunctive (dj) vs. conjunctive (cj) forms (based on Hyman & Watters 1984:251, taken from Hyman 2013; also Halpert 2016)

Examples of conjunctive and disjunctive forms in Xitsonga, isiZulu, Siswati, and Setswana are shown in (2). Descriptively speaking, the disjunctive form has a morpheme [(j)a] in all these languages, while the conjunctive form does not have this morpheme.

(2) Examples of conjunctive and disjunctive forms

In Xitsonga, the distinction between the disjunctive (dj) and conjunctive (cj) forms appears in the present positive. In terms of distribution, the cj form cannot appear in the clause-final position, while the dj form can appear in this position. Unlike in Zulu, various types of syntactic constituents can satisfy conditions for the cj form in Xitsonga. In other tenses, the morphological distinction between cj and dj forms is neutralised. Recent work on Setswana (Creissels 2014) argues that tonal distinctions are maintained in other tenses that are not present positive. We follow Creissels' observation and will show that a similar distribution is also found in Xitsonga.

The dj form is expressed with the -a- morpheme in Xitsonga. Due to the avoidance of vowel hiatus, there is vowel deletion (/ i + a / [a]), glide formation (/ u + a / [wa]), or vowel fusion (/ a + a / [a] without lengthening). In personal pronouns, the cj forms and the dj forms are as shown in (3 a) and (3b), respectively.

(3) Conjunctive vs. disjunctive forms in Xitsonga personal pronouns

Three major approaches aim to explain the distribution of cj versus dj forms in other Bantu languages. Van der Spuy (1993), Buell (2006), and Halpert (2016) propose a constituency-based approach, in which the dj form is argued to be realised when a verb lacks a complement. Drawing on Setswana, however, Creissels (1996) and subsequently Güldemann (2003) argue that the distribution of cj vs. dj forms is best to be analysed as a focus-driven phenomenon. Updating the focus-based approach, Creissels (2014) argues for an information packaging approach that relies on information theory to explain the cj/dj distribution. The last approach will also be defended in this paper.

The rest of the paper will examine each of these approaches in sections 2, 3, and 4, respectively. The non-morphological distinction of cj/dj forms in other tenses, where penultimate lengthening and tone show a non-segmental distinction, will be presented in section 5.

 

2. The constituency-based approach

The constituency-based approach states that the cj form is used "when a verb is followed by complements within IP", while the dj form appears when "a verb is IP final" (van der Spuy 1993) (see also du Plessis, Nxumalo and Visser 1995 and Buell 2006).

2.1 Data in support of the constituency-based approach

Xitsonga has much data that support the constituency-based approach. As a main diagnostic, the cj form does not appear clause-finally (4b), but the dj form can appear in the clause-final position (5a). When the dj form is used with a nominal complement (5b), the clause is interpreted as if the speaker is contradicting an earlier claim or assertion. In the rest of this paper, the addition of 'do' before a verb will indicate such a usage. An example corresponding to (5b) in Setswana is ungrammatical unless there is an object marker.

(4) No cj form in clause-final position

(5) No restriction on the distribution of the dj form

When the subject is non-pronominal, as in (6), the same distribution as in (4)/(5) is observed. The cj form cannot appear in the clause-final position.

(6) With a non-pronominal subject

The presence of the object pronoun is not sufficient; the cj form is still ungrammatical. The ungrammaticality of (7a) suggests that the cj form must be the rightmost element within an IP.

(7) The object pronoun and the cj/dj forms

When a prepositional phrase (PP) is present, indicating that the verb is no longer in the IP-final position, the cj form can be used, as in (8a).

(8) The presence of a manner PP

The cj form can also be used when a locative phrase is present. The verb is not IP-final in all of the examples in (9).

(9) The presence of a locative

The cj form (as well as the dj form) can be used with low adverbs such as ngopfu 'much', swo tala 'a lot', and kahle 'well', as in (10). This pattern is different from isiZulu and Setswana, where these adverbs cannot be used with the dj form.

(10) The presence of low adverbs

The cj form can also be used when temporal adverbs are present, as in (11). This is another example where the verb in Xitsonga is not treated as clause-final.

(11) The presence of a temporal adverb

ú dyá nàmu:ntlhà (cj) wá: dyà nàmù:ntlhà (dj) 'He eats/does eat today'

When an object NP is coupled with a co-referenced object prefix, however, it does not make the cj form grammatical, as shown in (12a). This means that the verb is clause-final, and the object NP is outside of that clause. The presence of an object prefix indicates that the object NP is not in its canonical position (Cheng & Downing 2009, Yoneda 2011 a.o.). As such, nyama in (12a) is seen as right-dislocated, which makes the verb dya clause-final; thus, the cj form is not allowed.

(12) Object prefix and object NP

Both cj and dj forms are allowed in the presence of an object NP followed by an adverb or a PP in (13). The verb is not located in the clause-final position in these examples.

(13) The presence of an object NP and adverb/PP

The presence of object agreement in (14) shows that the cj form is no longer grammatical, but the dj form is. Thus, the verb is clause-final. Note that penultimate lengthening patterns differ between (13b) and (14b). In (14b), the object prefix is lengthened, which furthermore suggests that the verb is clause-final. There is also a very short pause after the verb, which is not present in (13b).

(14) Object agreement prefix with canonical word order'

When the object NP is right-dislocated from its canonical position, an object prefix is obligatory. As shown in (15a,b), both cj and dj forms are grammatical, which suggests that the verb is not in the clause-final position. This is consistent with other examples (8-11) in which adjuncts also license the appearance of the cj form. The pattern in (15a) contrasts with (7 a), in which the cj form only has an object prefix in the absence of an NP complement. Moreover, the sentence in (15c) has the intended reading of a right-dislocated object NP in the absence of an adjunct. In such a case, the sentence is ungrammatical. The requirement of an adjunct occurring after the verb (compare (15a) with (15c)) further suggests that the verb must not be in the clause-final position when it is in the cj form.

(15) Right-dislocated object NP

In Xitsonga, sentences with a right-dislocated NP without object agreement are grammatical, unlike in many other Southern Bantu languages. As shown in (16), both the cj and dj forms are grammatical. This is different from isiZulu, in which the following sentences are reported to be ungrammatical (van der Spuy 1993:346). These sentences are ungrammatical in Setswana, too.

(16) No object prefix and a right-dislocated NP

In yes-no questions, the presence of the question morpheme blocks the cj form, which suggests that the verb is in the clause-final position.

(17) The question word xana

a. *u dya xana? (cj) b. wá: dyà xà:nà? (dj) 'Is he eating?'

So far, the data in this section have shown that the distinction between cj and dj forms can be explained using the constituency-based approach. The following section will present data that are potentially problematic for this approach.

2.2 Potentially problematic data for the constituency-based approach

In the future tense and the perfect tense, there is no morphological contrast between the cj form and the dj form, as shown in (18b,d,f,h). If the verb were clause-final, we would not expect the ungrammaticality of these examples. This contrast in the distribution of the dj form has also been noted in Baumbach (1987:221). We will revisit this issue in section 5.

(18) No morphological contrast in the dj form in the future and perfect tense

In the future tense, temporal adverbs can be fronted and only the cj form is allowed (19a,b). The morphological contrast is neutralised and the dj form causes ungrammaticality, as in (19c). The same pattern is found in past tense sentences (19d-f). Note that this pattern is the opposite of the pattern observed in the present tense (19g-i).

(19) Temporal adverbs

2.3 Summary

So far, we have seen the cj and dj forms in Xitsonga that support the constituency-based approach. In Xitsonga, the cj form cannot be used when a verb is in the clause-final position. The cj form can be used regardless of whether the following constituent is an argument or an adjunct. In non-present tenses, the cj form must be used, even when the verb is in the clause-final position.

The basic patterns of cj versus dj will be extended in the following section, where the distinction between the cj form and the dj form is taken to be focus-driven. Two competing hypotheses will be compared.

 

3. Focus-based approaches

3.1 Two hypotheses

Buell (2006) presents two hypotheses from earlier studies on the cj/dj distinction. The Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis is based on a study on Setswana (Creissels 1996). Under this hypothesis, the element following a cj form is in focus, while the element following a dj form is not in focus. As such, if the cj form is not followed by any other elements, the absence of focused elements is the source of ungrammaticality (see also Ndayiragije (1999) on Rundi and Sabel and Zeller (2006) on isiZulu wh-questions).

A competing focus-based approach is the Verb Focus hypothesis proposed in Güldemann (2003) and based on a study of isiZulu. Under this hypothesis, it is the verb itself that is in focus when the verb is in its dj form, while the verb is not in focus in its cj form.

The following subsections will examine Xitsonga counterparts to the isiZulu datasets explored in Buell (2006). As shown in Buell (2006), Xitsonga has both examples that support the focus-based approaches and examples that raise questions about these approaches.

3.2 The Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis

3.2.1 In support of the Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis

The response (R) to the wh-question (Q) in (20a) shows an example of postverbal focus. The postverbal adverb ehandle Outside' is focused, and only the cj form is allowed (see also van der Wal (2014) for morphosyntactic encoding of focus in Makhuwa and other Bantu languages). The dj form is not a possible response to the wh-question (20b).

(20) JJh-question and response

It is also the case that when the postverbal subject is the logical subject of the verb, the dj form is not allowed, as shown in (21b).

(21) Postverbal logical subjects

The distribution of wh-words such as yini 'what' and rini 'when' is restricted to the postverbal position (22). As reported in Setswana (Creissels 2014:7), the use of wh-words requires the cj form of the verb.

(22) The position of wh-words 'what', 'when', etc. is restricted to the postverbal position, and the cj form is obligatory

Xitsonga examples so far support the Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis. The cj form is required in examples where the element after the verb is focused.

3.2.2 Potential problems for the Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis

As in Buell's (2006) work on isiZulu, when other contexts are examined, Xitsonga also has examples that are problematic for the Postverbal Term Focus hypothesis. In (23a), the neutral-context question does not require an answer with focus. Even so, the cj form is required as a response (23b). The dj form (23c) is not a salient response.

(23) Neutral-context question

Resumptive pronouns are required in Xitsonga relative clauses. In (24c), the resumptive pronoun rona is used. A resumptive pronoun is not a focused element, but it is the cj form that is required in relative clauses. Moreover, the dj form is ungrammatical. In this sense, Xitsonga patterns with isiZulu (Buell 2006:18).

(24) Resumptive pronoun rona

Locative and temporal relatives also require resumptive pronouns in Xitsonga. Although there is no focused element following the verb, the cj form and not the dj form is required.

(25) Locative and temporal relatives

3.3 The Verb Focus hypothesis

3.3.1 In support of the Verb Focus hypothesis

Under the Verb Focus hypothesis, it is the verb itself that is argued to be in focus when the verb is in its dj form, while the verb is not in focus in its cj form (i.e. elements other than the verb are in focus). As shown in (26), the dj form has two meanings: one focused; one non-focused.

(26) The dj form with a verbal focus

a. ú tlángá éhà:ndlé 'He plays outside' (cj)

b. wá tM:ngà éhà:ndlé 'He plays/does play outside' (dj)

In Xitsonga, kahle 'well' can co-occur with the dj form, which suggests that the preceding verb is focused (27b). In isiZulu, sentences with kahle 'well' must have the cj form (cf. Buell 2006:18-19).

(27) Verbal focus and kahle 'well'

a. ndzi yímbélélá ká:htë Ί sing well' (cj)

b. ndzáyímbélélá ká:htë Ί do sing well' (dj)

Contrastive focus on the verb is also possible when sentences occur with kahle. While the cj form in (28a) means that the person sings better than he dances, in (28b), there is an additional focal meaning that emphasises the event of singing itself.

(28) Contrastive focus on verb

a. à ndzi cíní ká:htë, kàmbé ndzi yímbélélá ká:htë

'I don't dance well, but I sing well' (cj)

b. à ndzi cíní ká:htë, kàmbé ndzáyímbélélá ká:htë

'I don't dance well, but (when I do sing) I sing well' (dj)

3.3.2 A potential problem for the Verb Focus hypothesis

Buell (2006) also identifies a potential problem for the Verb Focus hypothesis. If the dj form signifies the verb focus, the cj form should not be allowed in verb focus cases. In (29), a relative resumptive pronoun is used, which suggests that the focused element in the sentence must be the verb nyíkílé 'gave'. Contrary to the prediction of the Verb Focus hypothesis, the cj form is used in (29a). Moreover, the dj form is ungrammatical.

(29) Relative resumptive pronoun and verb focus

3.4 Summary

An examination of focus-based approaches in light of Xitsonga data shows that some but not all distributional facts of the cj/dj forms can be accounted for. As Buell (2006) points out, the examples involving resumptive pronouns as well as the examples in non-focused contexts are non-trivial cases that require further explanation.

 

4. The information packaging approach

4.1 Information packaging

In a recent study, Creissels (2014:10) proposes that the cj/dj distinction can be best accounted for by information packaging theory. The cj form is used whenever the comment or verb phrase (VP) includes at least one element other than the verb itself, which implies that a cj verb form is followed by at least one phrase forming part of the comment, since the VP is strictly head-initial. The dj form is used whenever the comment/VP includes no element other than the verb itself, which implies that a dj verb form can only be followed by extraposed phrases that do not form part of the comment. This proposal is a departure from the structure-based approaches (section 2) and the focus-based approaches (section 3).

4.2 In support of the information packaging approach

Creissels (2014: 8) suggests a diagnostic for separating the cj form from the dj form.2 The na-construction has two meanings: 'with' and 'as well'. The distribution is shown in (30). The 'with' meaning of na can be used with both the cj and the dj form, whereas the 'as well' meaning can only be used with the dj form. There is also a difference in penultimate lengthening. The main verb shows penultimate lengthening in the dj form, but not in the cj form (see section 5 for more discussion).

(30) na-construction as a diagnostic (see Creissels 2014:8)

a. ndzl tlrhà náye:ná Ί work with him' (cj)

b. ndzá ti:rhà náye:ná 'I do work with him' (dj)

c. * ndzi tirha na mina (cj)

d. ndzá tvrhà ná ml:ná 'I work as well' (dj)

As in Creissels' diagnostics, the na-construction in Xitsonga can be disambiguated when the cj form or the dj form is used. When the cj form is used (31a), the 'with' interpretation is assigned. When the dj form is used (31b), the 'as well, too' meaning is assigned.

(31) The choice between cj and dj

a. Nsôvô ú tírha náyè: ná 'Nsovo works with her' (cj)

b. Nsôvô ti:rhà náyè:ná 'Nsovo works too' (dj)

In Setswana, Creissels (2014) reports that there is a distinction between 'too' and 'all'. In Xitsonga, this distinction is also found: (32b) na hina 'too' and (32c,d) hinkwerhu 'all'. Creissels analyses hinkwerhu as a comment about the topic hi 'we'. Thus, the cj form (in addition to the dj form) is allowed.

(32) 'too' versus 'all'

a. * hi tirha na hina (cj)

b. há ti:rhà ná hi:ná 'we work too' (dj)

c. hi tirhà hinkwè:rhù 'we all work' (cj)

d. hà ti:rhà hinkwè:rhù 'we all do work' (dj)

The second author provided contexts to disambiguate the sentences in (32). For example, let us assume that an inspector came to a workplace and found people who were lounging around. The inspector asks them xana na n'wina mi tirha laha 'Do you(pl.) work here too (=are you employed here too)?'. Then the addressees can protest and reply ha tirha na hina 'we work too' and use the dj form; the cj form is not available. However, if the inspector looks at the people who are working and asks them xana hi n'wina ntsena mi tirhaka 'Are you the only ones who work?', then the addressees people would respond with E-e, hi tirha hinkwerhu 'No, we all work' with the cj form. Note that the response E-e, ha tirha hinkwerhu 'No, we work too' with the dj form is also possible.

The inversion construction further supports the information packaging approach. In (33b), for example, 'Nsovo' is not a topic, but it is included in the VP. Thus, it is the cj form in (33a) that is grammatical, not the dj form. In (33c,d), the right-dislocated subject is an afterthought. As such, the cj form is ungrammatical (see also Creissels 2014:15).

(33) Inversion construction

a. kú tírhá Nsó:vo 'There works Nsovo' (cj)

b. * ku a tirha Nsovo (dj)

c. * u tirha, Nsovo (cj)

d. wá ti:rhà, Nsô:vô 'She works, Nsovo, that is' (dj)

A constituency analysis would explain why (33c) is ungrammatical: the right-dislocated subject is outside of a clause in (33d). However, it would have difficulty explaining why the dj form is not possible in (33b).

4.3 A potential problem for the information packaging approach

The information packaging approach predicts that only the cj form should be used with adverbs such as ngopfu 'much', swo tala 'a lot', and kahle 'well', because these adverbs are part of the VP and they cannot be topicalised (á la Creissels 2014:12). In Xitsonga, the dj form appears with these adverbs as well, which requires some updates to the information packaging approach.

(34) Manner adverbs

a. ú dyá ngó:pfu (cj) wá: dyà ngô:pfù (dj) 'He eats/does eat much'

b. ú dyá swô tá:la (cj) wá: dyà swô tá (dj) 'He eats/does eat a lot'

c. ú dyá ká:htë (cj) wá: dyà ká:htë (dj) 'He eats/does eat well'

 

5. The cj/dj distinction in other tenses: the role of prosody

As shown earlier, the dj form is not compatible with the perfect or the future tense. We maintain that the cj/dj distinction is still present, and we use penultimate lengthening as evidence of this distinction. In van der Spuy (1993:348-349), it has been reported that penultimate lengthening can be used to distinguish the cj and dj forms. Recall that van der Spuy's main proposal was based on syntactic constituency.

(35) Penultimate lengthening and the cj/dj forms

a. The penultimate vowel of the dj form is obligatorily lengthened.

b. The penultimate vowel of the cj form may not be lengthened.

Creissels (2014) also reports tonal distinctions between cj and dj in some tenses in Setswana. In Xitsonga, penultimate lengthening also plays an important role in the distinction between the cj and the dj forms, but tonal distinctions play a relatively minor role. Examples in this section will use the Creissels test ('with him' vs. 'too') to determine the presence of the distinction between the cj and dj forms.

5.1 Perfect and past

There is no morphological distinction between the cj form and the dj form in the perfect and past tenses. If we adopt the Creissels' test with na, the penultimate lengthening of the verb, however, distinguishes the cj form from the dj form: penultimate lengthening in the dj form (36b,d) vs. no such lengthening in the cj form (36a,c).

(36) The cj/dj form in the perfect and past tenses

a. ndzi tirhílé náyè: Ί have worked with him' (cj)

b. ndzi tirhí:lé ná mi:ná Ί have worked too' (dj) *ndza tirhile na mina

c. ndzi tshàmilé náyè: 'I have stayed with him' (cj)

d. ndzi tshàmi:lé ná mi:ná 'I too have stayed' (dj) *ndza tshamile na mina

Other forms in the perfect and past tenses also show the same distribution: perfect negative in (37), past in (38), and past negative (39).

(37) Perfect negative

a. à ndzi tírhángí náyè:^ 'I have not worked with him' (cj)

b. à ndzi tírhá:ngi ná mi:ná 'I have not worked either' (dj)

(38) Past

a. á ndzí tírhá náyè: 'I was working with him' (cj)

b. á ndzí ti:rhà ná mi:ná 'I was working too' (dj)

(39) Past negative

a. á ndzíngá tírhínáyé:nà 'I was not working with him' (cj)

b. á ndzíngá tí:rhi ná mi:ná 'I was not working either' (dj)

5.2 Future

Future tense also maintains the same distinction with penultimate lengthening. The cj form in (40a) has a single penultimate lengthening in the final element of the sentence, while the dj form in (40b) has two. Note that the morphological dj form of the subject pronoun ndza creates an ungrammatical sentence (40c). Recordings of the sentences with the dj form also show a pitch resetting at mi:na 'me', as in Figure 1. This distinction is also found in future negative (41), continuative future positive (42), and continuative future negative (43).

(40) Future tense

a. ndzi tà tirhà náyè:mi 'I will work with him' (cj)

b. ndzi tá ti:rhà ná mi:ná 'I will work too' (dj)

c. *ndza ta tirha na mina

(41) Future negative

a. á ndzíngá tirhi náyé:nà 'I will not work with him' (cj)

b. á ndzíngá tí:rhi ná mi:ná 'I will not work either' (dj)

(42) Continuative future positive (a ha ta)

a. ndzà há tá tirhà náyè:mi 'I will still work with him' (cj)

b. ndzà há tá ti:rhà ná mi:ná 'I will still work too' (dj)

(43) Continuative future negative.

a. à ndzi ngá há tirhi ná yé:nà 'I will not work with him any more' (cj)

b. à ndzi ngá há ti:rhi ná mi:ná 'I will not work either any more' (dj)

5.3 Progressive (le ku V-eni) and continuative (a ha V)

The present progressive in (44) shows the difference in penultimate lengthening as well. Due to the tonal spreading in Xitsonga, the tone of the verb's final syllable differs in the cj and the dj forms. The continuative present in (45) and (46) shows the same distribution.

(44) Present progressive

a. ndzi lé kú tirhèni náyè:ná 'I am working with him' (cj)

b. ndzi lé kú tirhè:ni ná mi:ná 'I am working too' (dj)

(45) Continuative present positive

a. ndzà há tirhà náyè:ná 'I am still working with him' (cj)

b. ndzà há ti:rhà ná mi:ná 'I am still working too' (dj)

(46) Continuative present negative

a. à ndzá há tirhi ná yé:nà 'I am not working with him any more' (cj)

b. à ndzá há ti:rhi ná mi:ná 'I am not working either any more' (dj)

5.4 Present negative

Unlike the present positive tense, the present negative does not have a morphological dj form. The absence of the dj form does not mean that there is no distinction between the cj form and the dj form. The dj form has an additional penultimate lengthening on the verb (47b,d,f), while the cj form does not have such lengthening (47a,c,e).

(47) The present negative also shows differences in penultimate lengthening

a. à ndzl tírhí ná yé:nà 'I don't work with him' (cj)

b. à ndzl tí:rhl ná ml:ná 'I don't work either' (dj)

c. à ndzl tsútsúmí ná yé:nà 'I don't run with him' (cj)

d. à ndzl tsútsú:ml ná ml:ná 'I don't run either' (dj)

e. à ndzlfámbí náyé:nà 'I don't walk with him' (cj)

f. à ndzlfá:mbl ná ml:ná 'I don't walk either' (dj)

5.5 Summary

Using Creissels' test, we maintain that tenses with no morphological distinction between the cj form and the dj form may still distinguish these forms using other prosodic cues. The additional presence of penultimate lengthening and pitch-raising in the dj form serves to make this distinction. We agree with a reviewer who pointed out that these prosodic cues could be a function of the pause that is found after the dj form. A deeper understanding of the nature between the cj/dj distinction and prosody in Xitsonga will be pursued in subsequent studies.

 

6. Conclusion

The distribution of the cj form and the dj form in Xitsonga mostly (but not always) follows patterns described in work on other Southern Bantu languages: the constituency approach, the focus-based approaches, and the information packaging approach.

However, none of these approaches provides a full explanation with respect to the distribution of Xitsonga cj/dj forms. It seems to us that one of the most reliable cues for the cj/dj forms might be penultimate lengthening. We suggest that issues regarding the module of prosodic structure formation need to be incorporated in future studies on the nature of the distribution of the cj/dj forms.

The dj form in Xitsonga has a different distribution than in other Southern Bantu languages: (a) the dj form can be used in sentences where the object NP is not right-dislocated (see 5b), and (b) constructions with a right-dislocated NP do not require an object prefix (see 16). As suggested by a reviewer, these differences will be clarified in future studies.

 

References

Baumbach, E. J. M. 1987. Analytical Tsonga grammar. Pretoria: UNISA.         [ Links ]

Buell, L. 2006. The Zulu conjoint/disjoint verb alternation: Focus or constituency? ZAS Papers in Linguistics 43: 9-30.         [ Links ]

Cheng, L. and L. Downing. 2009. Where is the topic in Zulu? The Linguistic Review 26(2-3): 207-238.         [ Links ]

Creissels, D. 1996. Conjunctive and disjunctive verb forms in Setswana. South African Journal of African Languages 16(4): 109-115.         [ Links ]

Creissels, D. 2014. Conjoint and disjoint verb forms in Tswana and other Bantu languages. Unpublished ms. Lyons: University of Lyons.         [ Links ]

Doke. 1927/1997. Textbook of Zulu grammar (6th ed.). Cape Town: Longman.         [ Links ]

du Plessis, J. A., N.E. Nxumalo and M. Visser. 1995. Tsongasyntax. Stellenbosch: Stellenbosch University Department of African Languages.         [ Links ]

Givón, T. 1972. Studies in ChiBemba and Bantu grammar (Studies in African Linguistics vol. 3). California: University of California Department of Linguistics and the African Studies Centre.         [ Links ]

Givón, T. 1975. Focus and the scope of assertion: Some Bantu evidence. Studies in African Linguistics 6: 185-205.         [ Links ]

Güldemann, T. 2003. Present progressive vis-à-vis predication focus in Bantu: A verbal category between semantics and pragmatics. Studies in Language 27(2): 323-360.         [ Links ]

Halpert, C. 2016. Argument licensing and agreement. Oxford: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Hyman, L. 2013. Disentangling conjoint, disjoint, metatony, tone cases, augments, prosody, and focus in Bantu (UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report). California: University of California, Berkeley.         [ Links ]

Klein, U. 2008. Conjunctive and disjunctive verb forms in Siswati. Unpublished ms. Stuttgart: University of Stuttgart.         [ Links ]

Meeussen, A.E. 1959. Essai de grammaire rundi. Tervuren: Musë Royal de l'Afrique Centrale.         [ Links ]

Ndayiragije, J. 1999. Checking economy. Linguistic Inquiry 30(3): 399-444.         [ Links ]

Sabel, J. and J. Zeller. 2006. Wh-question formation in Nguni. In J. Mugane, J.P. Hutchison and D.A. Worman (eds.) Selected Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. pp. 271-283.         [ Links ]

Sharman, J.C. 1956. The tabulation of tenses in a Bantu language (Bemba: Northern Rhodesia). Africa 25: 393-404.         [ Links ]

van der Spuy, A. 1993. Dislocated noun phrases in Nguni. Lingua 90: 335-355.         [ Links ]

van der Wal, J. 2014. Subordinate clauses and exclusive focus in Makhuwa. In R. van Gijn, J. Hammond, D. Matic, S. van Putten and A. V. Galucio (eds.) Information structure and reference tracking in complex sentences. John Benjamins. pp. 45-70.

Yoneda, N. 2011. Word order in Matengo (N13): Topicality and informational roles. Lingua 121(5): 754-771.         [ Links ]

 

 

* This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number 16K02641 and JSPS Invitation Fellowship for Research in Japan S16016. We also thank our research assistant Clementinah Burheni.
1 A reviewer points out that the -ile form in (18c,d) is similar to the isiZulu long-form marker for the perfect tense. In isiZulu, according to the reviewer, the cj perfect takes the - e suffix, while the dj perfect takes the -ile suffix. In Xitsonga, the - e suffix and the -ile suffix are variants of the perfect form; the - e suffix is mostly used in colloquial Xitsonga. As such, the morphological contrast observed in isiZulu does not occur in Xitsonga.
2 As pointed out by a reviewer, in Sesotho-Tswana languages, the full paradigm regarding tonal distinctions between cj and dj forms is much more complicated than the penultimate lengthening diagnostics explored here. A detailed tonal study of Xitsonga, however, is beyond the scope of the current paper.

Creative Commons License Todo o conteúdo deste periódico, exceto onde está identificado, está licenciado sob uma Licença Creative Commons