SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.106 número11 índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


SAMJ: South African Medical Journal

versión On-line ISSN 2078-5135
versión impresa ISSN 0256-9574

SAMJ, S. Afr. med. j. vol.106 no.11 Pretoria nov. 2016

http://dx.doi.org/10.7196/samj.2016.v106i11.12003 

IN PRACTICE
NON-COMMUNICABLE DISEASES

 

Addressing tobacco smoking in South Africa: Insights from behavioural science

 

 

G Ganz

MSocSci candidate. Department of Psychology, Faculty of Humanities, University of Cape Town, South Africa

Correspondence

 

 


ABSTRACT

Behavioural risk factors such as tobacco smoking contribute significantly to the global and local disease burden. This article surveys three behavioural science interventions that could reduce rates of tobacco smoking in South Africa.


 

 

In 2000, tobacco smoking contributed to 8% of mortality and 3.7% of disability-adjusted life-years in South Africa (SA).[1] The most recent study of prevalence found that 17.6% of South Africans smoke tobacco.[2] The social and economic costs of tobacco smoking are significant, and the importance of multipronged efforts to reduce rates of smoking cannot be overstated. Alone, public education campaigns which raise awareness of the health risks associated with smoking are unlikely to have a significant impact - this is because, more so than effortful thinking, health behaviours and decision-making are strongly determined by unconscious cognitive biases and heuristics.[3] Indeed, in the past decade there has been a rise in the use of behavioural science to address issues of public health.[4] Given that behavioural risk factors, such as smoking, contribute significantly to the global and local disease burden, evidence-based behavioural interventions have much to contribute to public health.[5] This article surveys three potentially cost-effective behavioural interventions that could reduce rates of tobacco smoking in SA.

 

Social norms

Perceived norms - what I think everyone else does and thinks - are a significant predictor of human behaviour.[6-8] An important finding from norms research is that we often overestimate the prevalence of and support for antisocial behaviours and underestimate those of pro-social behaviours.[8] Moreover, these misperceived norms often drive behaviour. For example, students at five schools in the USA significantly overestimated both how much bullying took place and how many people condoned bullying behaviour at their school. Increased misperception was correlated with increased bullying. An intervention that disseminated the accurate rates of prevalence and support reduced rates of bullying significantly.[9] Another social norms intervention in the USA significantly reduced rates of youth smoking initiation.'101 Since a minority (17.6%) of South Africans smoke tobacco, researchers could examine the actual and perceived norms of smoking, and construct interventions accordingly. These interventions could use nationally representative data such as the South African National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey[2] and disaggregate it into smaller areas in order to tap into salient social identities to disseminate relevant tobacco smoking norms. For example, a campaign could emphasise the fact that the vast majority of people in a particular area do not smoke, and that most people think smoking is unappealing.

 

Defaults

Default options refer to 'an option that will obtain if the chooser does nothing'.[11] Apart from cases where choice is mandated, default options are pervasive. Behavioural science research suggests that default options strongly influence choice outcomes in favour of the default.[3] For example, when a large corporation in the USA switched from voluntary (opt-in) to automatic (opt-out) enrolment in a retirement savings plan, the number of enrolled employees rose by 50%.[12]

The influence of defaults is attributed to a number of factors. Firstly, defaults are often seen as normative, or the recommended option.[8,11] Secondly, it takes effort to fill out forms or answer questions in order to make a decision. Thirdly, even when it does not take much physical effort, people tend to avoid making active decisions as they can be mentally taxing and cause unease.[13,14]

The default effect has an important application to the way in which we treat tobacco dependence. In SA, the treatment guidelines for tobacco dependence are as follows: '(i) identifying all smokers, alerting them to the harms of smoking and benefits of quitting; (ii) assessing readiness to initiate an attempt to quit; (iii) assessing the physical and psychological dependence to nicotine and smoking; (iv) determining the best combination of counselling/support and pharmacological therapy; (v) setting a quit date and providing suitable resources and support; (vi) frequent follow-up as often as possible via text/telephone or in person; (vii) monitoring for side-effects, relapse and ongoing cessation; and (viii) if relapse occurs, providing the necessary support and encouraging a further attempt when appropriate'. [15]

These guidelines are the norm in many countries around the world. Richter and Ellerbeck[14] point out that this particular treatment guideline deviates from the treatment of most chronic health conditions (including substance abuse) in that it is an opt-in treatment. In contradistinction, when a doctor diagnoses a patient with diabetes they do not assess the patient's readiness before initiating treatment - that is to say, ordinarily we take an opt-out approach to treatment. Shifting the treatment default for tobacco cessation from an opt-in to an opt-out system could have significant positive benefits for public health. The authors of a meta-analysis of tobacco smoking cessation interventions conclude that 'the evidence is sufficiently clear to recommend that doctors should offer support for cessation much more commonly that is currently the case, and prior assessment of willingness to quit excludes many who would have taken up the offer of assistance if offered it directly'.[16]

 

Packaging

In the same way that emotions are used by marketers and advertisers to attract consumers to a particular brand of cigarettes, they can also be used to discourage cigarette consumption.[4] Plain packaging coupled with graphic health warnings, which elicit disgust, provide another avenue to reduce tobacco smoking in SA. Australia's implementation of plain packaging and graphic health warnings in 2012 provides an important case study. A 1-year follow-up study found an increase in rates of intention to quit,[17] and another study found a 0.55% decrease in prevalence at 34 months post intervention.[18] Perhaps most noteworthy, a study found post-intervention decreases in the appeal of cigarettes packs to youth.[19] The SA government - which ratified the World Health Organization's Framework Convention on Tobacco Control in 2005 - has already expressed support for the introduction of plain packaging.[20]

 

Conclusion

Social norms, defaults, and packaging are three examples of the way in which behavioural science can contribute to issues of public health in SA. Behavioural interventions of this kind are amenable to testing in randomised controlled trials, which can help to determine their impact at scale and thereby influence policy. Researchers and policy-makers should focus their efforts on the role that behavioural science can play in developing cost-effective and evidence-based interventions to issues of public health in SA.

 

References

1. Groenewald P, Vos T, Norman R, et al. Estimating the burden of disease attributable to smoking in South Africa in 2000. S Afr Med J 2007;97(8):674-681.         [ Links ]

2. Reddy P, Zuma K, Shisana O, Jonas K, Sewpaul R. Prevalence of tobacco use among adults in South Africa: Results from the first South African National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. S Afr Med J 2015;105(8):648-655. DOI:10.7196/SAMJnew.7932        [ Links ]

3. Kahneman D. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux, 2013.         [ Links ]

4. Roberto CA, Kawachi I. Behavioral Economics and Public Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2015.         [ Links ]

5. Murray CJ, Lopez AD. Measuring the global burden of disease. N Engl J Med 2013;369(5):448-457. DOI:10.1056/nejmra1201534        [ Links ]

6. Cialdini RB, Kallgren CA, Reno RR. A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behaviour. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 1990;24(20):201-234. DOI:10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60330-5        [ Links ]

7. Cialdini RB, Demaine LJ, Sagarin BJ, Barrett DW, Rhoads K, Winter PL. Managing social norms for persuasive impact. Soc Influence 2006;1(1):3-15. DOI:10.1080/15534510500181459        [ Links ]

8. Tankard ME, Paluck EL. Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Soc Issues Policy Rev 2016;10(1):181-211. DOI:10.1111/sipr.12022        [ Links ]

9. Perkins HW, Craig DW, Perkins JM. Using social norms to reduce bullying: A research intervention among adolescents in five middle schools. Group Process Intergr Relat 2011;14(5):703-722. DOI:10.1177/1368430210398004        [ Links ]

10. Linkenbach J, Perkins HW. Most of us are tobacco free: An eight-month social norms campaign reducing youth initiation of smoking in Montana. In: Perkins HW, ed. The Social Norms Approach to Preventing School and College Age Substance Abuse: A Handbook for Educators, Counselors, and Clinicians. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 2003:224-234.         [ Links ]

11. Thaler RH, Sunstein CR. Nudge: Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth and Happiness. London: Penguin Books; 2009:93.         [ Links ]

12. Madrian BC, Shea DF. The power of suggestion: Inertia in 401(k) participation and savings behavior. Q J Econ 2001;116(4):1149-1187. DOI:10.1162/003355301753265543        [ Links ]

13. Johnson EJ, Goldstein D. Do defaults save lives? Science 2003;302(5649):1338-1339. DOI:10.1126/science.1091721        [ Links ]

14. Richter KP, Ellerbeck EF. It's time to change the default for tobacco treatment. Addiction 2015;110(3):381-386. DOI:10.1111/add.12734        [ Links ]

15. Van Zyl-Smit RN, Allwood B, Stickells D, et al. South African tobacco smoking cessation clinical practice guideline [Abstract]. S Afr Med J 2013;103(11):869-876. DOI:SAMJ.7484        [ Links ]

16. Aveyard P, Begh R, Parsons A, West R. Brief opportunistic smoking cessation interventions: A systematic review and meta-analysis to compare advice to quit and offer of assistance. Addiction 2012;107(6):1066-1073. DOI:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03770.x        [ Links ]

17. Durkin S, Brennan E, Coomber K, Zacher M, Scollo M, Wakefield M. Short-term changes in quitting-related cognitions and behaviours after the implementation of plain packaging with larger health warnings: Findings from a national cohort study with Australian adult smokers. Tob Control 2015;24(S2):ii26-ii32. DOI:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-052058        [ Links ]

18. Australian Government Department of Health. Post-Implementation Review: Tobacco Plain Packaging, 2016. https://ris.govspace.gov.au/files/2016/02/Tobacco-Plain-Packaging-PIR.pdf (accessed 20 August 2016).         [ Links ]

19. White V, Williams T, Wakefield M. Has the introduction of plain packaging with larger graphic health warnings changed adolescents' perceptions of cigarette packs and brands? Tob Control 2015;24(S2):ii42-ii49. DOI:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2014-052084        [ Links ]

20. News24.com. SA set to join global plain cigarette packaging trend. News24, 26 May 2016. http://www.news24.com/SouthAfrica/Local/Coastal-Weekly/sa-set-to-join-global-plain-cigarette-packaging-trend-20160525 (accessed 18 August 2016).         [ Links ]

 

 

Correspondence:
G Ganz
garyganz@gmail.com

Accepted 19 September 2016

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons