SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.42 número1A proverb in need is a proverb indeed: Proverbs, textbooks and communicative language abilityExploring female learners' perceptions of learning geometry in mathematics índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


South African Journal of Education

versión On-line ISSN 2076-3433
versión impresa ISSN 0256-0100

S. Afr. j. educ. vol.42 no.1 Pretoria feb. 2022

http://dx.doi.org/10.15700/saje.v42n1a1911 

ARTICLES

 

Experimentation anxieties of pre-school and primary school teacher candidates

 

 

Engin BaysenI; Fatma BaysenII

IDepartment of Classroom Teaching, Ataturk Faculty of Education, Near East University, Nicosia, Cyprus engin.baysen@neu.edu.tr
IIIndependent Researcher

 

 


ABSTRACT

Experimentation is widely accepted as being central to science education. However, anxiety about experimentation often prevents its use. While a considerable number of studies on anxiety related to laboratory experimentation have been conducted, studies on in-class experimentation is limited. In the research study reported on here we explored the anxiety about in-class experimentation of teachers as enacted in pre-school and primary school (PP) education. We also searched for possible similarities in the anxiety of the pre-school and primary school teacher candidate (TC) cohorts to explain the persistence of their anxiety. TCs responded to an open-ended question through a qualitative analysis procedure. The levels of anxiety were classified into three categories: low, moderate, and high. The time when the TCs experienced the anxiety was classified into 3 other categories: backstage, stage, and finale, which were coupled with the levels of anxiety. Quantitative analysis showed that there was a high similarity between the two cohorts of TCs' experimentation anxieties. We conclude that teacher candidates' future students would encounter a continuous, uninterrupted, unfavourable anxiety-filled environment with regard to experiments throughout their PP.

Keywords: anxiety; experimentation; pre-school; primary school; teacher candidates


 

 

Introduction

Experimentation is a method of inquiry that students should develop from the early years of pre-school and primary school (PP) education (Gutierrez, Cruz-Guzmán & Rodríguez-Marín, 2019). Experimentation is accepted as an important modality of teaching science (Docherty-Skippen, Karrow & Ahmed, 2020). Although a considerable amount of previous research argues against experimentation, for example, the cognitive load theory (Naude & Meier, 2019; Zhang, 2019), it is still a trusted teaching method in all formal education levels and scientific subjects. Experimentation holds many benefits. Some of these include developing observation, material manipulation, and communication skills. Moreover, experimentation increases one's knowledge of the environment, precise rules, and principles. It also increases self-efficacy in learning. Research has shown favourable attitudes toward the use of experiments in teaching. Teachers regard experimentation as the correct way to teach science. It is also a method that most students love (Nielsen, 2012). The need to experiment has become a cliché: a science course without experiments would be fruitless. Schools are equipped with experimentation materials and advocate cheap, straightforward, and easy-to-obtain apparatus (Yükseköğretim Kurulu, n.d.). Historically, the focus on experimentation has always existed; it is regarded as the primary key to engaging students in learning and increasing the number of students choosing to take science subjects. Recent science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) studies have opened a new perspective on experimentation by integrating STEM to solve problems. Thus, we regard the role of experimentation to be critical (Millî Eğitim Bakanlığı, 2018; Millî Eğitim Bakanlığı Eğitim Genel Müdürlüğü, 2013; Soares, De Campos, Thomaz, Da Cruz Pereira & Roehrs, 2016).

Regardless, stakeholders continue to complain about the limited numbers of students who choose science-related subjects and professions, and teachers who prefer not to use experiments in teaching. In our study we focused on the latter problem. Why don't PP teachers use experimentation as expected? Not conducting experiments in PP science classes is a critical problem. The reasoning behind this averse reaction to experiments includes inefficacy in coping with anxieties (Pendergast, Lieberman-Betz & Vail, 2017).

A great deal of research has considered the issue of anxiety in the teaching of science. However, few studies only considered experiments as the cause of anxiety. Literature either includes experimentation as a part of science anxiety creation (Bryant, Kastrup, Udo, Hislop, Shefner & Mallow, 2013) or focuses on laboratory issues (Alkan & Erdem, 2013) and assesses problems with laboratory equipment, student engagement and whether teachers can use the laboratory efficiently or not. Thus, most research addresses secondary and upper grades but not PP education. However, PP teachers mostly use experiments in the class instead of in laboratories. They either do experiments in class or tell their students to do so. PP teachers either perform demonstration experiments or let their students conduct simple experiments (Çepni & Ayvacı, 2015:292-294).

With this study we aimed to address this gap in the literature by attempting to elucidate the issue at the PP education level. There is a need to understand the context of PP education to produce more systematic and methodologic discussions and suggestions. Thus, it is necessary to gain deeper insight into PP teacher candidates' (TCs) experimentation anxiety before designing a teacher training programme. Instead of adopting the findings of laboratory anxiety and findings of higher education levels to the PP education level, there is a need to assess the context of experimentation in PP education only to conduct a more appropriate discussion. Expecting of TCs to solve their anxiety-related problems themselves seems unrealistic. Moreover, experienced teachers can increase new teachers' anxiety (Shemwell, Avargil & Capps, 2015). Thus, teacher trainers do not have the luxury of ignoring the anxiety issue. Teacher trainers should do their best to prevent the development of anxiety in TCs. They should also alleviate this anxiety if it already exists. Furthermore, depending on the science curriculum, TCs and pupils should be fond of inquiry-based science and experimentation. Teachers should have a positive attitude toward experiments; at least, their anxiety should not be so high that it prevents them from using experiments in their teaching. Additionally, they should not transfer their anxiety to their students. In our study we focused on the experiment-related anxiety of PP education teachers. The study benefits teachers, TCs and teacher trainers.

Background

Anxiety

Researchers have attempted to enlighten readers on the phenomenon of anxiety. Anxiety is inevitable, and it originates from threats or perceived threats (Downing & Filer, 1999; Jones, Hobbs, Kenny, Campbell, Chittleborough, Gilbert, Herbert & Redman, 2016). Fear of the unknown and of failure and a lack of information and skills can easily cause anxiety (Shen, 2008). Anxiety is mostly perceived as a problem. The level of deterioration depends on the experiences of anxiety (Bandura, 1989; Jasalavich, 1992); these experiences can cause burnout (Byrne, 1994), which leads to the loss of skills and can prevent effort and innovation (Shen, 2008). Being conservative prevents apprehension (Bonwell & Eison, 1991:57).

In addition, those experiencing anxiety can stop, decrease, or prevent trouble (Bandura, 1989). There are ways to prevent or eliminate the causes of anxiety (Alkan & Erdem, 2013; Bursal, 2012; Cox & Carpenter, 1989; Gilbert & Byers, 2017; Palmer, 2006). Bandura highlights that high self-efficacy can allow an individual to overcome or reduce anxiety (Bandura, 1989), which in turn leads to achievement (Zoller & Ben-Chaim, 1989). However, research has shown that anxiety persists in practice (Bandura, 1989; Kaplan, 1970; Yürük, 2011).

Teaching anxiety

Anxiety influences teaching environments; it creates problems for teachers and students (Beisel, 1991; Gilbert & Byers, 2017; Koran & Koran, 1981), and it also influences teaching strategies (Czerniak & Haney, 1998; Czerniak & Schriver, 1994; Keavney & Sinclair, 1978; Sinclair & Nicoll, 1980). Teachers with high anxiety, particularly new teachers, do not choose the teaching methods that they were taught in teacher training, but rather they choose the methods with which they feel most comfortable. Any attempt to change their methods may create anxiety, which is not welcomed by teachers nor their students (Bonwell & Eison, 1991; Woods, 1994). Teachers often choose the same method that their PP education schoolteachers chose or what is recommended by older teachers. Consequently, they choose teacher-centred approaches (Czerniak & Haney, 1998; Hodgin, 2014).

Research has shown that the causes of anxiety are related to different dimensions of the learning environment (Çelik, 2008). These dimensions include managing the class (Eksi & Yakis.ik, 2016), a lack of subject knowledge (Rice, 2005), undergoing personal evaluation (Morton, Vesco, Williams & Awender, 1997), having the skill to deal with unexpected cases (Alpan, Özer, Erdamar & Subas.i, 2014), and using time effectively (Nair & Ghanaguru, 2017).

Researchers have often included demographic variables in examining influences on anxiety. Morton et al. (1997) found that anxiety was culture-dependent. They found that there were similarities in the levels of anxiety in different countries. When individuals shared similar cultures, i.e., Canadian and British TCs often displayed similar anxieties. Student teachers' anxiety increase as they progress from early to later primary classes. Female teachers are more anxious than male teachers.

As in our study, researchers have sought to categorise anxiety in order to increase their comprehension thereof. Çelik (2008) categorised anxiety of preservice teachers of English as a foreign language (EFL). He categorised anxiety into the following six categories: personal, communication-centred, evaluation-based, external, lesson-preparation and teaching-related anxiety. He also categorised anxiety into three levels, namely, low, medium, and high.

Another categorisation concerns the progress from teacher candidate to teacher. Fuller and Bown (1975) were the pioneering researchers in creating stage theory for concern. According to Fuller and Bown, TCs' focus on anxiety changes over time. It develops along the route from teacher candidates becoming teachers. They note three types of concern, namely, "self-concern", "task concern", and "impact concern." Self-related anxiety may include the approval of other teachers, that of students, and that of other school-related individuals. Task-related concern includes class and time management. Impact concern includes meeting students' learning needs.

Many researchers agree with Fuller's stage development theory (Beeth & Adadan, 2006; Conway & Clark, 2003; Malderez, Hobson, Tracey & Kerr, 2007; Pyper, 2009; Van den Berg, Sleegers & Geijsel, 2001). Others are of the opinion that the stage and chronological approaches to teacher concerns are not valid (Capel, 2001; Evans & Tribble, 1986; Rogan, Borich & Taylor, 1992). According to these studies, different anxieties can exist at the same time.

Teacher anxiety is correlated with student anxiety (Beilock, Gunderson, Ramirez & Levine, 2010; Doyal & Forsyth, 1973). This consequence is the most damaging as it shows that anxiety can be transferred to students, which has a snowball effect and adds to the status quo.

Science and experimentation anxiety

Most research views teachers' knowledge, self-efficacy beliefs and anxiety towards science, inquiry, and laboratory use as important because these issues may create obstacles in science education (Harlen & Holroyd, 1997; Hodson, 1993; Jarvis & Pell, 2004; Karakaya, Avgın & Kumperli, 2016; Osborne, Simon & Collins, 2003; Van Driel, Beijaard & Verloop, 2001). Research states that there is a negative correlation between the level of anxiety related to teaching science and self-efficacy (Yürük, 2011). Teachers are expected to reserve more time for science activities and thereby benefit from it (Czerniak & Lumpe, 1996; Westerback & Long, 1990). Theoretically, increasing the time spent on teaching science would increase the experimentation time. Nevertheless, research has shown that teachers spend less time on science than on other subjects (Blank, 2013; Brand & Wilkins, 2007; Fulp, 2002; Roychoundhury, 1994).

Recognising the lack of the use of experimentation, researchers have tested different approaches to decrease their anxiety about science and experimentation. For example, Palmer (2006) used primary science methods and, more recently, Ural (2016) used a guided inquiry laboratory approach with third-year, undergraduate science education students as an intervention. Both authors found a decrease in anxiety. Although the researchers used the idea that occupying students with first-hand experimentation activities would prevent experimentation anxiety, a lack of research about the PP education context still exists.

Purpose

To improve science teaching, the first step in diminishing the effect of anxiety or preventing the causes of anxiety is to understand the nature thereof. Exploring their origins and the interrelations among the causes of anxiety is essential. For example, we examined how TCs experience anxiety, how they transfer it to others, the reasons behind the persistence of anxiety and how anxiety influences TCs' science teaching. Thus, we aimed to discover more in-depth information about the anxiety related to experiments. We attempted to answer the following questions:

1) How can experiment-related anxiety be categorised depending on

a) its levels of influence and

b) the time it is experienced?

2) Is there any relation between the levels of anxiety and the time that TCs experience it?

3) Is there any relation between the types and levels of anxiety of the two cohorts of TCs?

 

Methods

The research study reported on here was based on mixed methods research (Teddlie & Tashakkori, 2009:7). A qualitative research method was adopted. Qualitative research seeks to reveal in-depth information about a particular group of people. The categories that emerged were quantified to reveal the differences (if any) between the two groups of TCs (Chi, 1997). The first two research questions were answered using qualitative research procedures. Parts 3A (types) and 3B (levels) of the last question depended on statistical procedures based on quantitative research.

Participants

One hundred and ten pre-school (n = 68) and primary school (n = 42) Turkish TCs participated in the study. Thus, two different cohorts of TCs were included in the study to answer the third research question, with which we tried to reveal a predictor for the persistence of anxiety. The pre-school cohort included 62 female and six male TCs. The primary school cohort comprised 32 female and 10 male TCs. The TCs came from different parts of Turkey, but mainly the eastern and south-eastern geographical regions. The participating TCs were 20 or 21 years old. Both of these cohorts of TCs would teach science-related subjects during their careers. Generally, TCs should prepare their students in a way that is compatible with the standards of upcoming grade levels. Those standards depend on constructivist approaches. Constructivist approaches accept inquiry and experimentation as cornerstones. The participating pre-school TCs were in their fifth semester while the primary school TCs were in their third semester of the training programmes that comprised a total of eight semesters. Both TC groups were students at the same university. The pre-school TCs were enrolled for the Pre-school Science Education course while the primary school TCs were enrolled for the Science and Technology Laboratory course.

Data Collection and Ethical Issues

The data were collected during the 2018-2019 fall semester at one of the private universities in northern Cyprus where Turkish is spoken and accepted as a teaching language. The data were collected at the beginning of the semester in ordinary classes. Each TC responded to her/his question individually within a predetermined time. TCs used one class period to answer the question. The lessons continued normally after all the TCs had responded. The same open-ended question was posed to both TC cohorts: "What are your concerns about utilising experiments in your teaching? Explain your answer based on your experiences." The question was presented on a blank page, and the TCs could use extra pages if required. One of the researchers was present in the class while the students were responding. He answered questions when they were raised and checked the submitted answers for possible misunderstanding. Only a few responses were returned to the students to add more detail.

The researchers explained the aim of the study and the importance of the study for the TC's future education. The students who participated in the study participated voluntarily and their consent for participation was obtained from them. The TCs were informed that their responses would be used for scientific reasons only and that the responses would remain anonymous. Furthermore, they were informed that they could withdraw from the study at any time during the research process and that their responses would then be excluded from the study (Creswell, 2012:592).

Data Analysis

The data analysis was carried out using qualitative research analysis methods (Creswell, 2012:236-264; Marton & Booth, 1997:132-136). The analysis included five steps.

Both researchers prepared the data and repeatedly read the answers to take note of the experiment-related anxiety among the TCs.

The two researchers read the data a number of times more times to determine the dimensions (level and timing of anxiety) that depended on the most common responses stated by the participants. These would be the focus of researchers and readers worldwide and would be beneficial for studies on science anxiety. Ideas that the TCs seldom mentioned were omitted. The list of coding dimensions appears in Appendix A.

The two researchers together read the code list many times to assign categories to the dimensions that were clearly different from each other, had logical relations with each another, and of which there were only a few (Marton & Booth, 1997:124-125).

An independent researcher experienced in qualitative research was asked to perform the coding for inter-rater reliability (Creswell, 2012:161). The analysis was finalised only after an acceptable ratio had been reached. The inter-rater reliability was calculated using Holsti's method (Neuendorf, 2002:167-190). The formula is as follows: PAo= 2A/(nA+nB), where PAo is the proportion agreement observed, A is the number of agreements between the two coders, and nA and nB are the numbers of units coded by coders A (researchers in this study) and B (independent researcher). Because the numbers of units coded by the coders were equal at 20 and the number of agreements between the coders was 19 for the levels of anxiety and 20 for the timing, the PAos were 0.95 and 1.00, respectively. Thus, the PAos (> 0.80) were highly acceptable.

Descriptive and inferential analysis methods were used in the quantitative part of the study. The analysis included frequencies, percentages, and chi-square calculations. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) 21 was used for chi-square calculations.

 

Results

TCs stated different kinds of experiment anxiety, which were all generated in secondary and high school classes. TCs put forward two dimensions in their responses to the open-ended question, namely, the level of anxiety and the time at which this particular anxiety was prominently experienced. TCs talked about different interference levels of experiment anxiety. Anxiety interferes with one's functioning when experimenting. That is, the more severe the anxiety, the more likely it is that the anxiety will interfere with the teachers' willingness to follow through on experimentation in the classroom.

The levels of anxiety were not predetermined but deducted from the TCs' responses in that the TC's responses included some indication of their anxiety levels. For example, "I should practice many times to ensure that I am not unsuccessful"was categorised as low anxiety because the interference level was lower than in the following two examples. "If I recognise that my preparation was not fruitful (low anxiety) and I could not reach scientific results (moderate anxiety), then the problem is big!" indicates that the anxiety is greater than that in the previous example. "I cannot bear hurting my students. It is better to not do experiments"was categorised as high anxiety. Three categories of anxiety levels thus emerged: low, moderate, and high (see Appendix A).

We also considered the time when the TCs had experienced the particular anxiety. The time dimension had a chronological logic. The time of the anxiety was not predetermined but rather deducted from the TC's responses. The anxiety started from the moment that a TC decided to perform an experiment, continued while the experiment was being conducted, and ended at the final moment when the experiment ended. We named these time intervals the backstage, stage, and finale, respectively. For example, we categorised "a lack of subject knowledge" as being backstage anxiety because the TCs stated that they experienced this feeling before the experiment.

The time dimension was found to accompany the level of anxiety (see Figure 1). Three links were thus formed. Low anxiety was linked to backstage, moderate anxiety was linked to stage, and high anxiety was linked to finale. It was evident that there was an increase in the level of anxiety from backstage through stage to the finale.

In the following section we present more detailed findings including the categories for the dimensions, the characteristics of the categories, and the relations between the categories, explaining how the categories were interrelated and decided upon.

Low Anxiety (L)

The low anxiety category included encountering problems caused by a lack of certain aspects that were required for using experiments in science teaching. These aspects (drawbacks) were knowledge, skills, experience, expert support, preparation, and exercise, where knowledge included knowledge of the materials, knowledge of the learners, and knowledge of the experiment. Low anxiety was matched by the backstage period, and it was about predictable, manageable, and preventable situations. TCs can resolve this level of anxiety beforehand through a particular amount of study, experience, and attention. These anxieties did not cause critical danger but rather lead to a dreadful start. This category of concern did not stop teacher candidates from using experiments. Thus, this category represented a challenging situation but still indicated a positive mental state.

Moderate Anxiety (M)

This category was about making mistakes during the experiment and included many concerns (see Appendix A). The stage period matched this level of anxiety. Overall, moderate anxiety comprised uncertainty about experimenting. Thus, these anxieties may ruin the experiment and cause non-scientific results. Again, these consequences were not vital. However, they were more critical than those included in the preceding category. This category represented a challenging and semi-pessimistic state of mind. Moderate anxiety may be coupled with other low anxiety issues and emerge as a more significant problem. Additionally, these may create a pessimistic mood resulting in TCs giving up on using experiments in their teaching. One TC stated: "If I recognise that my preparation was not fruitful (low anxiety) and I could not reach scientific results (moderate anxiety), then the problem is big!"

High Anxiety (H)

High anxiety included the risk of hurting others and facing humiliation, and these issues were based on critical dangers during experiments. For example, one TC, talking about her secondary, school stated: "What if the mixture explodes?"

This rationale stemmed from experience. One TC stated: "While experimenting, I heated the tube; it exploded and I was afraid."Another TC emphasised the event by sharing the following anecdote: "My teacher in secondary school lost one of his fingers while experimenting."

The high anxiety level represented the ultimate fear of the experiment. It was the final uncontrollable phase of the experiment. This level could end any experiment or experimenting as a teaching approach, which caused TCs to refrain from doing future experiments. One of the TCs said: "Since then (secondary school), I did not even intend to experiment."This category included a total pessimistic character. The TCs in this category confessed that they would not use experiments in their classes since they could not handle being responsible for their students: "I cannot bear hurting my students. It is better to not do experiments; I can teach them using other methods instead."

Only three TCs (one from pre-school and two from primary school) did not show any anxiety (see Table 1). Thus, almost all TCs showed anxiety with regard to using experiments in teaching. Nearly half of the TCs had a high level of anxiety. Approximately 80% of TCs had multiple levels (two or three) of concern.

The issue of a lack of expert support was noted in the pre-school (f = 5) group but not in the primary group; it was categorised as a low level of anxiety. Only five pre-school TCs noted the issue of "managing the class",and it was categorised as a moderate level of anxiety. Moreover, only one primary school TC noted the issue of "patience."No particular high-level anxiety was detected in only one group. The results did not show a significant difference between the two cohorts. Thus, there was a need to quantify the data and conduct an inferential analysis.

There was a significant difference between the number of TCs with experiment anxiety (f = 107) and those without experiment anxiety (f = 3). A chi-square test delivered no significant difference between the number of TCs who had high anxiety (f = 58) and those who did not (f = 52), χ2 (1, N = 110) = .327, p > .05. Moreover, there was no significant difference between the two cohorts, χ2 (1, N = 110) = .203, p > .05. Finally, the gender variable did not result in any significant difference either, χ2 (1, N = 110) = 98.327, p > .05.

 

Discussion

In this section we discuss the findings of our research, state the implications for science teaching and education in general, indicate its contribution to theory, and make suggestions for future research. By assessing TCs' anxiety, we hope that teacher trainers, TCs, teachers, and researchers benefit from this study.

The dimensions of categories of anxieties revealed in the study will benefit teacher trainers and teachers in general. The teacher trainers and teachers need to understand the types of anxiety that their students experience in order to define the problem and act accordingly. For example, if the anxiety is related to backstage, then teacher trainers can understand that the anxiety is related to a lack of preparation for the experiment, and that the TC did not appropriately practice the experiment. In addition, if it is about the finale and fear of humiliation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), the teacher trainer can create an atmosphere of acceptance for the TC to gain "social success" (Kaplan, 1970:75). Moreover, teacher trainers should not underestimate the cases of low and moderate anxiety; they should pay attention and prevent their co-occurrence since the combination may increase the perceived threat.

The different levels of experiment-related anxiety, particularly the three categories raised, were consistent with the literature findings on science teaching (Çelik, 2008). TCs had experiences that influenced their attitudes about experiments. These experiences accumulated to create a hopeless situation. This outcome is a type of "learned helplessness." The result is that their experiment experiences could prevent TCs from conducting future experiments (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette & Strosahl, 1996; Raufelder, Regner & Wood, 2018).

Lazarus (1991) state that people may react to anxious situations in three ways. The first is avoidance, the second is to remain in the situation but to increase one's vigilance, and the last is to eliminate the source of the threat. In the high-anxiety situation about the use of experiments, teacher trainers should suggest the second and third responses, as the worst choice for the TCs in our study was to avoid or not do experiments at all.

An attempt at experiments during teaching could spark multiple and different types of anxiety. If a TC overcomes any one type of anxiety, another type of anxiety may appear. Other than the level of anxiety, the persistence of experiment-related anxiety also had a disturbing effect, which resulted in teachers refraining from experimenting in their careers. One TC clearly stated: "Let's say I prepared well ... gathered the right materials for the experiment. Then, what if a student had an accident? I cannot control them all."

There was no significant difference between the two cohorts of TCs, which supports the findings of culture dependency (Morton et al., 1997). Moreover, there was no significant difference between the two cohorts with regard to gender as a variable, which is consistent with the findings of Karakaya et al. (2016). If TC exhibit experiment anxiety once they start teaching, this anxiety may be transferred to their students, resulting in the transfer and continuation of anxiety with regard to experimentation.

One important finding of this study is that TCs stated that the experiences that caused their anxiety took place in secondary and high school, but not in the pre-school or primary school. Teacher trainers should ensure that TCs understand that the goals of pre-school and primary school teachers differ from those of secondary and high school teachers. PP education requires more fundamental skills, knowledge, and materials. Thus, these teachers would need more straightforward, manageable, and less dangerous experiments, and there would be no need to fear PP education experiments.

Many studies (Boz, 2008; Capel, 2001; Fuller & Brown, 1975; Matoti & Lekhu, 2016) indicate that class management is a cause of anxiety. However, this was not evident from the responses in our study.

The findings regarding features of experimentation anxieties in our study contribute to the Theory of Inquiry (Cox & Carpenter, 1989) in science teaching, particularly in a teacher training context, as experiments are at the heart of inquiry. Experimentation anxieties

interfere in varying degrees

originate in context (in secondary and high school) but can still influence other situations (in PP)

feed each other and create a bigger issue

increase as the stage approaches

are culturally dependent and thus persist. Although challenges regarding the teaching of science in primary schools in the South African context exist (James, Beni & Stears, 2019), there is a lack of research dealing with science anxiety. For example, Matoti and Lekhu (2016) dealt with pre-service teachers' sources of anxiety in a general sense and stated that classroom management was regarded as an anxious experience. We hope that our study will result in researchers focusing on anxiety and anxiety in the teaching of science in South Africa and similar contexts in order to solve problems related to anxiety in teaching.

In our study we dealt with pre-school and primary school TCs, which enabled us to contrast their respective levels of anxiety - one of the strengths of the study. We suggest that future research should be done on primary-secondary and secondary-higher TCs to make similar comparisons.

 

Conclusion

Most TCs experience experiment-related anxiety. Experiment-related anxiety depends on the level of the influence of the causes and their timing. The categories of low, moderate and high anxiety match the time periods of backstage, stage, and finale, respectively. Both the level and persistence of anxiety throughout secondary and high school harm one's general attitude towards experimenting. The combinations of different levels of anxieties result in a more complicated and difficult problem. The finale stage is the most critical because it keeps teachers from conducting experiments.

 

Authors' Contributions

The research was conceptualised by Engin Baysen and Fatma Baysen. Engin Baysen was responsible for the data collection. Engin Baysen and Fatma Baysen drafted, critically revised and finalised the article for publication.

 

Notes

i. Published under a Creative Commons Attribution Licence.

 

References

Alkan F & Erdem E 2013. The effect of self-directed learning on the success, readiness, attitudes towards laboratory skills and anxiety in laboratory. Hacettepe Unuiversity Journal of Education, 44:15-26.         [ Links ]

Alpan G, Özer A, Erdamar G & Subaşı G 2014. The development of a student teacher concerns scale. Eurasian Journal of Education Research, 54:151-170. Available at https://dergipark.org.tr/en/download/article-file/59884#page=153. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Bandura A 1989. Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44(9): 1175-1184. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.9.1175        [ Links ]

Beeth ME & Adadan E 2006. The influences of university-based coursework on field experience. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 17:103-120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10972-006-9013-8        [ Links ]

Beilock SL, Gunderson EA, Ramirez G & Levine SC 2010. Female teachers' math anxiety affect girls' math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(5):1860-1863. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0910967107        [ Links ]

Beisel RW 1991. An antidote for science anxiety. Science and Children, 29(2):35-36.         [ Links ]

Blank RK 2013. Science instructional time is declining in elementary schools: What are the implications for student achievement and closing the gap? Science Education, 97(6):830-847. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21078        [ Links ]

Bonwell C & Eison JA 1991. Active learning: Creating excitement in the classroom. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report No. 1. Washington, DC: The George Washington University, School of Education and Human Development. Available at https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED336049. Accessed 7 April 2018.         [ Links ]

Boz Y 2008. Turkish student teachers' concerns about teaching. European Journal of Teacher Education, 31(4):367-377. https://doi.org/10.1080/02619760802420693        [ Links ]

Brand BR & Wilkins JLM 2007. Using self-efficacy as a construct for evaluating science and mathematics methods courses. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 18:297-317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10972-007-9038-7        [ Links ]

Bryant FB, Kastrup H, Udo M, Hislop N, Shefner R & Mallow J 2013. Science anxiety, science attitudes, and constructivism: A binational study. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 22:432-448. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-012-9404-x        [ Links ]

Bursal M 2012. Changes in American preservice elementary teachers' efficacy beliefs and anxieties during a science methods course. Science Education International, 23(1):40-55. Available at https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ975549.pdf. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Byrne BM 1994. Testing for the factorial validity, replication, and invariance of a measuring instrument: A paradigmatic application based on the Maslach Burnout Inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 29(3):289-311. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2903_5        [ Links ]

Capel S 2001. Secondary students' development as teachers over the course of a PGCE year. Educational Research, 43(3):247-261. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131880110081026        [ Links ]

Çelik M 2008. Türkiye'deki ingilizce öğretmen adaylarının uygulama dersinde yaşadıkları endişe ve kaygılar [Pre-service EFL teachers' reported concerns and stress for practicum in Turkey]. Egitim ve Bilim, 33(150):97-109.         [ Links ]

Çepni S & Ayvacı HS 2015. Laboratuar destekli fen ve teknoloji öğretimi [Laboratory supported science and technology teaching]. In S Çepni (ed). Kuramdan uygulamaya fen ve teknoloji öğretimi [Science and technology teaching from theory to practice]. Ankara, Turkey: Pegem Akademi.         [ Links ]

Chi MTH 1997. Quantifying qualitative analyses of verbal data: A practical guide. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6(3):271-315. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327809jls0603_1        [ Links ]

Conway PF & Clark CM 2003. The journey inward and outward: A re-examination of Fuller's concern-based model of teacher development. Teaching and Teacher Education, 19(5):465-482. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(03)00046-5        [ Links ]

Cox CA & Carpenter JR 1989. Improving attitudes toward teaching science and reducing science anxiety through increasing confidence in science ability in inservice elementary school teachers. Journal of Elementary Science Education, 1(2): 14-34.         [ Links ]

Creswell JW 2012. Educational research: Planning, conducting, and evaluating quantitative and qualitative research (4th ed). Boston, MA: Pearson.         [ Links ]

Czerniak CM & Haney JJ 1998. The effect of collaborative concept mapping on elementary preservice teachers' anxiety, efficacy, and achievement in physical science. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 9(4):303-320. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009431400397        [ Links ]

Czerniak CM & Lumpe AT 1996. Relationship between teacher beliefs and science education reform. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 7(4):247-266. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00058659        [ Links ]

Czerniak CM & Schriver ML 1994. An examination of preservice science teachers' beliefs and behaviors as related to self-efficacy. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 5(3):77-86. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02614577        [ Links ]

Docherty-Skippen SM, Karrow D & Ahmed G 2020. Doing science: Pre-service teachers' attitudes and confidence teaching elementary science and technology. Brock Education Journal: A Journal of Educational Research and Practice, 29(1):25-35. Available at https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1242702.pdf. Accessed 8 June 2020.         [ Links ]

Downing JE & Filer JD 1999. Science process skills and attitudes of preservice elementary teachers. Journal of Elementary Science Education, 11(2):57-64. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173838        [ Links ]

Doyal GT & Forsyth RA 1973. Relationship between teacher and student anxiety levels. Psychology in the Schools, 10(2):231-233. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(197304)10:2<231::AID-PITS2310100219>3.0.CO;2-W        [ Links ]

Dweck CS & Leggett EL 1988. A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95(2):256-273.         [ Links ]

Ekşi GY & Yakışık BY 2016. To be anxious or not: Student teachers in the practicum. Universal Journal of Educational Research, 4(6):1332-1339. https://doi.org/10.13189/ujer.2016.040610        [ Links ]

Evans ED & Tribble M 1986. Perceived teaching problems, self-efficacy, and commitment to teaching among preservice teachers. Journal of Educational Research, 80(2):81-85.         [ Links ]

Fuller F & Bown O 1975. Becoming a teacher. In K Ryan (ed). Teacher education: The seventy-fourth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.         [ Links ]

Fulp SL 2002. 2000 National Survey of Science and Mathematics Education: Status of elementary school science teaching. Chapel Hill, NC: Horizon Research. Available at http://www.horizon-research.com/horizonresearchwp/wp-content/uploads/2013/04/elem_science.pdf. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Gilbert A & Byers CC 2017. Wonder as a tool to engage preservice elementary teachers in science learning and teaching. Science Education, 101(6):907-928. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.21300        [ Links ]

Gutierrez MP, Cruz-Guzmán M & Rodríguez-Marín F 2019. Prospective early childhood teachers' difficulties in analysing children's ideas about the natural and social environment. South African Journal of Education, 39(2):Art. #1608, 10 pages. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v39n2a1608        [ Links ]

Harlen W & Holroyd C 1997. Primary teachers' understanding of concepts of science: Impact on confidence and teaching. International Journal of Science Education, 19(1):93-105. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069970190107        [ Links ]

Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM & Strosahl K 1996. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(6):1152-1168. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1152        [ Links ]

Hodgin CM 2014. Science teaching anxiety: The impact of beliefs on teacher preferences of instructional strategies. PhD dissertation. Austin, TX: The University of Texas at Austin. Available at https://repositories.lib.utexas.edu/handle/2152/247 44. Accessed 25 February 2018.         [ Links ]

Hodson D 1993. Philosophic stance of secondary school science teachers, curriculum experiences and children's understanding of science: Some preliminary findings. Interchange, 24(1-2):41-52. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01447339        [ Links ]

James AA, Beni S & Stears M 2019. Teaching science in the foundation phase: Where are the gaps and how are they accounted for? South African Journal of Childhood Education, 9(1):a759. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajce.v9i1.759        [ Links ]

Jarvis T & Pell A 2004. Primary teachers' changing attitudes and cognition during a two-year science in-service programme and their effect on pupils. International Journal of Science Education, 26(14):1787-1811. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069042000243763        [ Links ]

Jasalavich SM 1992. Preservice elementary teachers' belief about science teaching and learning and perceived sources of their beliefs prior to their first formal science teaching experience. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Cambridge, MA, 21-25 March.         [ Links ]

Jones M, Hobbs L, Kenny J, Campbell C, Chittleborough G, Gilbert A, Herbert S & Redman C 2016. Successful university-school partnerships: An interpretive framework to inform partnership practice. Teaching and Teacher Education, 60:108-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2016.08.006        [ Links ]

Kaplan BL 1970. Anxiety-A classroom close- up. The Elementary School Journal, 71(2):70-77. Available at https://www.jstor.org/stable/1000518. Accessed 11 May 2020.         [ Links ]

Karakaya F, Avgın SS & Kumperli E 2016. Analysis of primary school student's science learning anxiety according to some variables. Journal of Education and Practice, 7(33):24-31. Available at https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1122604.pdf. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Keavney G & Sinclair KE 1978. Teacher concerns and teacher anxiety: A neglected topic of classroom research. Review of Educational Research, 48(2):273-290. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543048002273        [ Links ]

Koran JJ, Jr & Koran ML 1981. The effects of teacher anxiety and modeling on the acquisition of a science teaching skill and concomitant student performance. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 18(4):361-370. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660180411        [ Links ]

Lazarus RS 1991. Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.         [ Links ]

Malderez A, Hobson AJ, Tracey L & Kerr K 2007. Becoming a student teacher: Core features of the experience. European Journal of Teacher Education, 30(3):225-248. https://doi.org/10.1080/02619760701486068        [ Links ]

Marton F & Booth S 1997. Learning and awareness. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.         [ Links ]

Matoti SN & Lekhu MA 2016. Sources of anxiety among pre-service teachers on field placement experience. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 26(3):304-307. https://doi.org/10.1080/14330237.2016.1185921        [ Links ]

Millî Eğitim Bakanlığı 2018. Fen bilimleri dersi öğretim programı: İlkokul ve ortaokul 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 ve 8. Siniflar [Science lesson curriculum: Primary and secondary school 3, 4, 5, 6, 7 and 8th grades]. Ankara, Turkey: Author. Available at http://mufredat.meb.gov.tr/ProgramDetay.aspx?PID=325. Accessed 8 June 2020.         [ Links ]

Millî Eğitim Bakanlığı Eğitim Genel Müdürlüğü 2013. Okul öncesi eğitimi programı[Preschool education program]. Available at http://tegm.meb.gov.tr/dosya/okuloncesi/ooproram.pdf. Accessed 8 June 2020.         [ Links ]

Morton LL, Vesco R, Williams NH & Awender MA 1997. Student teacher anxieties related to class management, pedagogy, evaluation, and staff relations. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67(1):69-89. https://doi.org/10.1.2044-8279.1997.tb01228.x        [ Links ]

Nair P & Ghanaguru S 2017. Owning the classroom: Student teacher's experiences and concerns during practicum. The English Teacher, 46(3):142-159. Available at http://www.journals.melta.org.my/TET/downloads/tet46_03_04.pdf. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Naude M & Meier C 2019. Elements of the physical learning environment that impact on the teaching and learning in South African Grade 1 classrooms. South African Journal of Education, 39(1):Art. #1342, 11 pages. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v39n1a1342        [ Links ]

Neuendorf KA 2002. The content analysis guidebook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.         [ Links ]

Nielsen BL 2012. Science teachers' meaning-making when involved in a school-based professional development project. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 23:621-649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10972-012-9300-5        [ Links ]

Osborne J, Simon S & Collins S 2003. Attitudes towards science: A review of the literature and its implications. International Journal of Science Education, 25(9):1049-1079. https://doi.org/10.1080/0950069032000032199        [ Links ]

Palmer DH 2006. Sources of self-efficacy in a science methods course for primary teacher education students. Research in Science Education, 36:337-353. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-005-9007-0        [ Links ]

Pendergast E, Lieberman-Betz RG & Vail CO 2017. Attitudes and beliefs of prekindergarten teachers towards teaching science to young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 45:43-52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-015-0761-y        [ Links ]

Pyper J 2009. Pre-service teacher efficacy trajectories. Paper presented at the Canadian Society for the Study of Education annual conference, Ottawa, Canada, 23-26 May.         [ Links ]

Raufelder D, Regner N & Wood MA 2018. Test anxiety and learned helplessness is moderated by student perceptions of teacher motivational support. Educational Psychology, 38(1)54-74. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2017.1304532        [ Links ]

Rice DC 2005. I didn't know oxygen could boil! What pre-service and in-service elementary teachers' answers to 'simple' science questions reveals about their subject matter knowledge. International Journal of Science Education, 27(9):1059-1082. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690500069426        [ Links ]

Rogan JM, Borich GD & Taylor HP 1992. Validation of the stages of concern questionnaire. Action in Teacher Education, 14(2):43-48. https://doi.org/10.1080/01626620.1992.10462810        [ Links ]

Roychoundhury A 1994. Is it minds-off science? A concern for the elementary grades. Journal of Science Teacher Education, 5(3):87-96. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02614578        [ Links ]

Shemwell JT, Avargil S & Capps DK 2015. Grappling with long-term learning in science: A qualitative study of teachers' views of developmentally oriented instruction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 52(8): 1163-1187. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21239        [ Links ]

Shen Y 2008. The effects of changes and innovation on educational improvement. International Education Studies, 1(3):73-77. Available at https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1065412.pdf. Accessed 2 June 2018.         [ Links ]

Sinclair K & Nicoll V 1980. The sources and experience of anxiety in practice teaching. Washington, DC: ERIC Clearinghouse.         [ Links ]

Soares BC, De Campos MEC, Thomaz JR, Da Cruz Pereira G & Roehrs R 2016. The importance of experimentation in the teaching of sciences to elementary school. Revista Monografias Ambientais - REMOA, 15(2):1-17. https://doi.org/10.5902/2236130827003        [ Links ]

Teddlie C & Tashakkori A 2009. Foundations of mixed methods research: Integrating quantitative and qualitative approaches in the social and behavioral sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.         [ Links ]

Ural E 2016. The effect of guided-inquiry laboratory experiments on science education students' chemistry laboratory attitudes, anxiety and achievement. Journal of Education and Training Studies, 4(4):217-227. https://doi.org/10.11114/jets.v4i4.1395        [ Links ]

Van den Berg R, Sleegers P & Geijsel F 2001. Teachers' concerns about adaptive teaching: Evaluation of a support program. Journal of Curriculum and Supervision, 16(3):245-258.         [ Links ]

Van Driel JH, Beijaard D & Verloop N 2001. Professional development and reform in science education: The role of teachers' practical knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 38(2):137-158. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2736(200102)38:2%3C137::AID-TEA1001%3E3.0.CO;2-U        [ Links ]

Westerback ME & Long MJ 1990. Science knowledge and the reduction of anxiety about teaching earth science in exemplary teachers as measured by the science teaching State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. School Science and Mathematics, 90(5):361 -374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1949-8594.1990.tb17225.x        [ Links ]

Woods DR 1994. Problem-based learning: How to gain the most from PBL. Waterdown, Canada: Donald R. Woods.         [ Links ]

Yükseköğretim Kurulu n.d. Fen bilgisi öğretmenliği: Lisans pogramı[Science teaching: Undergraduate program]. Available at https://www.yok.gov.tr/Documents/Kurumsal/egitim_ogretim_dairesi/Yeni-Ogretmen-Yetistirme-Lisans-Programlari/Fen_Bilgisi_Ogretmenligi_Lisans_Programi.pdf. Accessed 12 March 2018.         [ Links ]

Yürük N 2011. The predictors of pre-service elementary teachers' anxiety about teaching science. Journal of Baltic Science Education, 10(1):17-26. Available at http://www.scientiasocialis.lt/jbse/files/pdf/vol10/17-26.Yuruk_Vol.10_No.1.pdf. Accessed 28 February 2022.         [ Links ]

Zhang L 2019. "Hands-on" plus "inquiry"? Effects of withholding answers coupled with physical manipulations on students' learning of energy-related science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 60:199-205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.01.001        [ Links ]

Zoller U & Ben-Chaim D 1989. Interaction between examination type, anxiety state, and academic achievement in college science; an action-oriented research. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 26(1):65-77. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660260107        [ Links ]

 

 

Received: 21 July 2019
Revised: 11 December 2020
Accepted: 8 February 2021
Published: 28 February 2022

 

 

Appendix A

 


Appendix A - Click to enlarge

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons