SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.39 issue3"They should know where they stand": attitudes to HIV Voluntary Counselling and testing amongst a group of out-of-school youthCreative thinking in prospective teachers: the status quo and the impact of contextual factors author indexsubject indexarticles search
Home Pagealphabetic serial listing  

Services on Demand

Article

Indicators

Related links

  • On index processCited by Google
  • On index processSimilars in Google

Share


South African Journal of Education

On-line version ISSN 2076-3433
Print version ISSN 0256-0100

S. Afr. j. educ. vol.39 n.3 Pretoria Aug. 2019

http://dx.doi.org/10.15700/saje.v39n3a1622 

ARTICLES

 

Relationship between prospective teachers' deontic justice attitudes and academic dishonesty tendencies

 

 

Sait AkbaşlıI; Şefika Şule ErçetinI; Sevda KubilayII

IEducation Faculty, Hacettepe University, Ankara, Turkey. sakbasli@gmail.com
IIVocational School of Social Sciences, Nigde Omer Halisdemir University, Nigde, Turkey

 

 


ABSTRACT

The aim of the study was to determine the relationsip between deontic justice attitudes among prospective teachers and their tendencies towards academic dishonesty. Participants in the study were 403 university students attending different departments of the Education Faculty of the Hacettepe University, a state university in the Central Anatolia Region of Turkey. The sample was determined by means of a stratified sampling technique, and students were chosen randomly from different departments. Attitudes towards deontic justice and tendencies towards academic dishonesty among the participants were stratified in terms of gender and students' affiliation to a specific department of education. The findings suggested that female students and participants from the English Language Education Department had the highest deontic justice attitudes; male students and participants from the Physical Education and Sports Department had the highest tendencies towards academic dishonesty. A low, negative and statistically significant relationship between deontic justice attitudes and academic dishonesty tendencies exists.

Keywords: academic dishonesty tendency; deontic justice attitude; prospective teachers


 

 

Introduction

Justice and ethics persist as central concerns throughout human history (Cropanzano, Stein & Goldman, 2007). Justice as a concept can not be limited to individual matters; it also concerns the way in which others are treated, and the reactions of people towards events by which they are not directly affected (Skarlicki & Kulik, 2005). Justice is more than reasonableness (Du Preez & Simmonds, 2011).

According to the notion of deontic justice, fairness is considered as an end in itself and is perceived as a moral virtue, independent from the group-based identity and personal interest (Beugré, 2010). Justice is also related to behaviour motives and social behaviour (Lerner, 1975). Therefore, in this study, the concept of deontic justice was paired with academic dishonesty tendencies. Academic dishonesty involves different forms of cheating behaviour, which occur with different motives. Orosz, Dombi, Tóth-Király, Bőthe, Jagodics and Zimbardo (2016) claim that students' perspective of time influences their cheating behaviours. Having longterm goals may hinder students from cheating or display academic dishonesty, whereas seizing the day and low academic motivation may result in different levels of cheating behaviour. In addition, a tendency towards dishonesty in referencing and research may stem from insufficiencies in the education system and students' lack of knowledge about the requisite research stages. These are the motives known to push students to behave dishonestly. This study questioned whether the perception of justice affected the students' academic dishonetsy behaviours or not, so the relationship between the concept of deontic justice and the tendencies towards academic dishonesty were examined. Prospective teachers were chosen as a target group because learning to teach is a complex, interactive, and dynamic situation, affected by individual and contextual variables (Quick & Siebörger, 2005). Another reason for choosing prospective teachers as participants was that the attitudes of educators play an important role in reducing the cheating behaviour of students. If educators do not care about cheating, students tend to show more cheating behaviour (Gresley, Wallace, Hubb & Staats, 2009). Moral values and academic integrity should therefore be promoted by educators (McCabe, Trevino & Butterfield, 2001).

In such a dynamic situation it is crucial to determine the possible motives for teacher dishonesty. It is believed that this study will have cross-border importance, because the concept of justice is universal and not region-specific, implying possible value in raising awareness among educators globally.

Literature Review

Justice is generally explained based on the motives behind it, and three models that reflect different perspectives about motives exist. These are the instrumental model, the relational model, and the deontic model. The instrumental model proposes that individuals prefer justice in pursuit of favourable outcomes where they can exercise long-term control over the results. The relational framework emphasises the relationship between an individual and the social group s/he values. According to this view, fair treatment indicates respect and regard towards a particular person. Although the first two models are supported by research, neither seems to explain every dimension of justice (Cropanzano, Massaro & Beckers, 2017). These models have arguably ignored moral obligations and have pointed essentially to personal aspirations as the reason for behaving in a negative way to justice and injustice. More recent studies have included a moral dimension and emphasise that justice is not only about economic interests or group-based identity, but also related to that which people regard to be ethically appropriate. In this vein, Folger (2001) developed the concept of deontic justice to express the extent to which moral responsibility and sense of responsibility are related to judicial judgements and actions. A deontic justice viewpoint takes correctness as a moral virtue. At the basis of the concept is the notion of caring about others. For any behaviour to be perceived as fair, it is expected that the conduct is not only fair to the individual, but to others as well (Beugré, 2012).

Despite extensive research related to ethics and moral justice, dishonesty is seemingly progressively more prevalent in the world today. Academic dishonesty is a pervasive problem throughout the world (Blankenship & Whitley, 2000). According to O'Neill and Pfeiffer (2012) one's moral compass affects students' actions: if students believe cheating to be ethically unacceptable, they are reluctant to become involved in any cheating behaviour. Students' perceptions about cheating and the reaction of society towards those engaged in cheating also influence the frequency of the behaviour. The way in which students perceive cheating is therefore crucial in determining whether they will engage in or refrain from academic dishonesty.

Deontic justice

Deontic justice proposes that justice is important for its own sake and that standards of fairness exist. According to this view, people care for justice towards everyone, even when they are not directly involved. They follow principled moral obligations - even when the obligations do not serve their own self-interest - because they value the standards of justice (Cropanzano et al., 2017). However, in daily life people do not always pay attention to the mistreatment that others face due to the difference between the social groups to which they belong (Greene, 2013). When people are concerned about the fair treatment of other people, regardless of their social group, it is possible to infer deontic justice. The basic principal of deontic justice is to care for oneself and others; behaviour is considered ethically appropriate only when it conforms to moral norms that are valid not only for oneself, but also for others (Folger, 2001). The concept of deontic justice has multiple dimensions, namely, moral obligation, moral accountability, and moral outrage. Moral obligation is conceptualised as an experience of a moral imperitive to act according to moral norms and being consistent with moral principles. Moral accountability is the desire to hold offenders responsible for unfair conditions when witnessing actions violating justice. Moral outrage refers to the negative feelings, anger and resentment when witnessing unfairness, and an effort to restore unfairness (Beugré, 2012). Deontic justice is therefore related to the socialised and internalised values of the individual (Lau & Wong, 2009); it comprises the moral obligation to conform to moral norms, holding those lacking morality accountable for their misdeeds, and experiencing discomfort when observing a violation of justice.

Academic dishonesty

Academic dishonesty can take various forms of cheating, such as copying during exams, letting someone else look at one's paper, writing crib notes, copying assignments, and plagiarism (O'Neill & Pfeiffer, 2012). Lin and Wen (2007) argue that academic dishonesty is related to the desire of students to obtain higher grades and certain interests. According to Odabaşi, Birinci, Kiliçer, Şahin, Akbulut and Şendağ (2007), the reasons for academic dishonesty in referencing include the difficulty students have in citing a source in their native language, tiring procedures of preparing a bibliography, and not questioning the source of online materials. In his study on the cheating motives of prospective mathematics teachers, Eraslan (2011) revealed that participants considered some courses to be unnecessary, therefore they chose an easy way to pass the course. Eraslan (2011) also suggests that a teaching and learning system based on memorising and retrieval of information causes academic dishonesty and cheating behaviour. Tsui and Ngo (2016) emphasise the relationship between students and their peers where a student's positive attitude toward his/her peers' cheating behaviours may affect the frequency of his/her own cheating actions. In that vein, educators' attitudes are also important. Ersoy and Özden (2011) found that students' tendency towards plagiarism from the internet is related to the attitude of the instructor; if the instructor is strict about the issue, the tendency towards dishonest conduct is lower.

Certain demographic features are also influential in predicting one's inclination towards cheating. Studies have revealed that males are generally more likely to cheat than females (Dawkins, 2004). There is also a negative correlation between academic dishonesty and academic performance; successful students are less likely to cheat during their education (Burrus, McGoldrick & Schuchmann, 2007; Dawkins, 2004; Finn & Frone, 2004; Karim & Ghavam, 2011). Moral beliefs and values are also crucial factors that hinder students to engage in cheating (Eraslan, 2011). Although many studies pertaining to the frequency of academically dishonest behaviour have been done, studies on factors that may predetermine a tendency toward academic dishonesty among students may play an important role in preventing misbehaviour (Eminoglu & Nartgün, 2009). This study was aimed at determining the relationship between a deontic justice attitude and a tendency towards academic dishonesty among prospective teachers. It was hypothesised that - as deontic justice requires of people to have a sense of moral obligation and accountability - individuals with a deontic justice attitude are likely to display less of a tendency towards academic dishonesty. Although studies on the tendencies towards academic dishonesty among students exist, studies specifically addressing the relationship between an attitude of deontic justice and a tendency towards academic dishonesty could not be found. It was therefore believed that this research could contribute to the field, by addressing the following research questions:

· Do deontic justice attitudes of prospective teachers show significant differences according to their gender and department of education?

· Do academic dishonesty tendencies of prospective teachers show significant differences according to their gender and department of education?

· Does a relationship exist between deontic justice attitudes and academic dishonesty tendencies of prospective teachers?

 

Methodology

This research was a quantitative and descriptive study conducted to explore deontic justice attitudes and tendencies towards academic dishonesty among a cohort of prospective teachers. Quantitative research aims to determine the relationship between an independent and a dependent variable, and a descriptive study establishes associations between them (Hopkins, 2008). To this aim, participants were asked to indicate their gender and the department of education they were involved with because it was believed that these variables could potentially affect the deontic justice attitudes and academic dishonesty tendencies of the prospective teachers. Both the attitudes and the tendencies were measured with standardised measurement scales and the relationship between these was statistically determined.

The participants comprised 403 university students attending the Education Faculty of a state university located in the Central Anatolia Region, in the 2016-2017 education year. The population consisted of students who continued their education in one of seven teaching departments in the faculty: Social Sciences Education, Primary Mathematics Education, Turkish Education, English Language Education, Science Education, Physical Education and Primary Education. The sample was determined with 95% confidence level and 0.03 sampling error (Yazıcıoğlu & Erdoğan, 2004). The stratified random sampling method was applied so that the entire population was divided into strata, which were the departments in the Education Faculty. For this study the departments were considered as homogeneous groups. The proportionate stratification method was implemented to adequately represent each subgroup. In other words, the sample size of each department was ensured to be proportionate to the poplulation size of the department. After determining the strata sample size, the simple random sampling method was applied to each stratum to select participants for the study. Voluntary participation was always considered imperative and before research surveys were distributed to students, each potential participant was reminded that s/he could withdraw from the study whenever s/he wanted, and that their personal information would remain anonymous, that their answers would be confidential and used only for scientific research. The participants only received the scales once they had agreed to take part. Two scales, the Deontic Justice Scale and the Academic Dishonesty Tendency Scale, were applied in this research. Prior to the research, consent to use the scales was gained via email. The implementation process was completed in the Education Faculty where the participants were requested to complete the questionnaires distributed to them. The participants'demographic features are reflected in Table 1.

 

 

Female participants represented 71% of the sample (n = 286) and male participants 29% (n = 117). Participants were distributed across all seven departments within the Education Faculty with the lowest number of participants in the Primary Mathematics Education Department (7.7% of the sample) and the highest number in the English Language Education Department (23.6% of the sample).

Beugré (2012) developed the Deontic Justice Scale to measure the properties related to deontic justice. The validity and reliability study of the Turkish version of the deontic justice scale was undertaken in 2013 (Akın, Sarıçam, Kaya, Akdeniz, Gediksiz, Toprak & Yıldız, 2013). The scale comprises 18 items and three sub-scales. These sub-scales are moral obligation, which has eight items, moral accountability, with six items, and moral outrage with four items. Each item is rated according to a typical five-level Likert rating scale, namely: (1) strongly disagree, (2) disagree, (3) neither agree nor disagree, (4) agree, and (5) strongly agree. The alpha internal consistency coefficients for the overall scale were found to be .87, and for the sub-scales .75, .77, and .85 respectively (Akın et al., 2013).

Eminoglu (2008) developed the Academic Dishonesty Tendency Scale to measure the academic dishonesty tendency of university students. It consists of 22 items and four sub-scales: tendency towards cheating with five items; tendency towards dishonesty in assignments, and studies such as projects, which has seven items; tendency towards dishonesty in the process of research and report with four items; dishonesty tendency towards ascriptions with six items. Responses were evaluated on a five-level Likert-type scale: (1) strongly agree; (2) disagree; (3) neither agree nor disagree; (4) disagree; and (5) strongly disagree. The Cronbach alpha reliability coefficents of the overall scale are .90 and .71, .82, .78, and .77 for the sub-scales respectively.

Participants' scores on the Academic Dishonesty Tendency Scale and Deontic Justice Scale were treated as dependent variables; participants' gender and departments of education were treated as independent variables. The demographic features of the participants were analysed with the use of descriptive statistics. An independent samples t-test was applied to explore a potential difference in deontic justice mean scores or academic dishonesty tendency mean scores between male and female participants. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and the TUKEY test were performed to make sense of the possible differences in mean scores between each of the seven departments of education. Lastly, correlation analysis was done to reveal meaningful relationships between any dimension of deontic justice and any dimension of academic dishonesty tendency; also between deontic justice and academic dishonesty tendency as unidimensional constructs. Data was analysed with version 16.0 of the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS).

 

Findings

The study aimed to shed light on the deontic justice attitudes and the academic dishonesty tendencies of a sample of prospective teachers. Demographic variables - gender and the specific education department that participants were attached to - were taken into consideration and examined to determine whether either of these were related to the dependent variables in any way. The possibility of a relationship between deontic justice attitudes and tendencies towards academic dishonesty was also examined.

Deontic Justice Attitudes and Gender

To determine whether any relationship existed between gender and one's deontic justice attitude, an independent samples t-test was performed of which the results are presented in Table 2.

 

 

Table 2 indicates that there are no significant differences in the mean scores between male and female participants for the following: deontic justice (combined score), moral obligation (sub-scale), and moral accountability (sub-scale). However, in terms of moral outrage, the mean score of female participants is higher than that of males ( = 17.63 and = 16.91 respectively) (t-test = 2.25; p < .05) implying a meaningful difference between the average scores of the two groups. P-value indicates whether a difference is statistically significant or not. If the value is lower than 0.05, it is accepted as significant (Pallant, 2010). Here, the female group's p-value is 0.026, which is lower than 0.05, so the difference is statistically significant.

Deontic Justice Attitudes and Affiliation

To determine whether a differentiation in deontic justice scores of students in terms of their departments existed, ANOVA was applied. The results show that students from the English Language Education Department had the highest combined score for deontic justice. The variance analysis was then performed to determine differences between and within the groups.

When Table 3 is examined, the F value (F = 3.034; p < .05), related to moral outrage and the F value (F = 2.349; p < .05) for deontic justice (combined score) indicates that there is a statistically significant difference between groups in each respective case. The p-value for the moral outrage score between groups is 0.007, which is lower than 0.05; similarly the p-value for deontic justice between groups is 0.031, which is also lower than 0.05, means that there is a statistically significant difference between groups.

 

 

As there was evidence that group means differed, the TUKEY test was performed to investigate which of the means were different. When more than two groups are used, the TUKEY test compares the difference between each pair of means. The results of the TUKEY test are reported in Table 4.

With reference to the sub-scale, moral outrage, the p-value (p = 0.022; p < .05) shows that there is a significant difference between participants affiliated to the English Language Education Department and participants related to the Primary Mathematics Education Department. The moral outrage attitude of participants from the English Language Department is significantly higher than that of participants from the Primary Mathematics Education Department. With reference to the deontic justice combined score, there is a significant difference (p = 0.018; p < .05) between participants from the English Language Education Department and participants from the Science Education Department. Overal deontic justice attitudes among participants from the English Language Education Department are significantly higher than those of participants from the Science Education Department.

Academic Dishonesty Tendencies and Gender

To determine whether the academic dishonesty tendencies of female and male participants differed, an independent samples t-test was applied, of which the findings are reported in Table 5.

Table 5 indicates that the tendency towards academic dishonesty among females - with regard to overall dishonesty and in relation to each of the sub-scales - is significantly lower than among males. T-test scores in each instance confirm this finding, with scores ranging from (t = -2.189; p < .05) in the case of cheating and (t = -4.275; p < .05) in the case of dishonesty in assignments and projects. The p-value of female participants for each sub-scale is lower than 0.05, which is an indicator for statistical significance.

Academic Dishonesty Tendencies and Affiliation

To determine whether there was a differentiation in academic dishonesty tendency scores of students in terms of their departments, ANOVA was performed. The results show that students from the Physical Education and Sports Department have the highest combined score of academic dishonesty tendency. The variance analysis was then performed to determine differences between and within groups.

Table 6 displays the variance in the scores for academic dishonesty tendency among participants related, firstly, to their own groups and secondly, among all groups.

 

 

From Table 6 it is clear that significant differences in the scores of participants' tendencies towards cheating (F = 6.885; p < .05), tendency towards dishonesty in studies such as assignments, projects, etc. (F = 3.462; p < .05), dishonesty tendency towards ascriptions (F = 5.946; p < .05), and dishonesty overall (F = 7.161; p < .05) are found between groups. Apart from the sub-scale of tendency towards dishonesty in the process of research and reporting, for all sub-scales the p-value is lower than 0.05, which indicates the significancy.

As there was evidence that group means differed, the TUKEY test was performed to investigate which of the means were different and the results are presented in Table 7.

The TUKEY test results confirm the relatively high tendencies towards academic dishonesty among participants from the Physical Education and Sports Department. The tendency to cheat is significantly higher than that of participants from any other department. Their tendency towards dishonesty in assignments and projects is significantly higher that that of participants from the English Language Education Department. The tendency to dishonesty in ascriptions is significantly higher than that of participants from the Turkish Education Department and the English Language Department. The tendency towards academic dishonesty overall (combined score) is significantly higher than that of participants from the Turkish Education Department, the Social Sciences Education Department, and the English Language Education Department. The positive outcomes of the analysis for the Language Education Department are noticeable at face level.

The Relationship Between the Deontic Justice Attitudes and Academic Dishonesty Tendencies of Prospective Teachers

Table 8 presents the correlation between the deontic justice attitudes of the participants and their tendencies towards academic dishonesty.

Table 8 reveals the following statistically relevant correlations between the different dependable variables:

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between moral obligation and moral accountability (r = .695), moral outrage (r = .621), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = .920)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between moral obligation and cheating (r = -.105), dishonesty in research and reporting (r = -.173), dishonesty in ascriptions (r = -.169), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = -.171)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between moral accountability and moral obligation (r = .695), moral outrage (r = .632), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = .889)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between moral accountability and dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = -.115), dishonesty in research and reporting (r = -.148), dishonesty in ascriptions (r = -.177), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = -.167)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between moral outrage and moral obligation (r =. 621), moral accountability (r = .632), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = .801)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between moral outrage and cheating (r = -.109), dishonesty in research and reporting (r = 127), dishonesty in ascriptions (r = -.206), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = -.165)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between the tendency towards cheating and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = .722)

· moderate, positive, and statistically significant relationship between the tendency towards cheating and dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = .464), and dishonesty in research and reporting (r = 357)

· low, positive, and statistically significant relationship between cheating and dishonesty in ascriptions (r = .271)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between cheating and moral obligation (r = -.105), moral outrage (r = -.109), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = -.113)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in assignments and projects and dishonesty in research and reporting (r = .573), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = .842)

· moderate, positive, and statistically significant relationships between dishonesty in assignments and projects and cheating (r = .464), and dishonesty in ascriptions (r = .496)

· low, positive, statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in assignments and projects and moral accountability (r = -.115), and deontic justice (r = -.111)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in research and reporting and dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = .573), dishonesty in ascriptions (r = .514), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = .757)

· moderate, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in research and reporting and cheating (r = .357)

· low, negative, and statistically significantly relationship between dishonesty in research and reporting and moral obligation (r = -.173), moral accountability (r = -.148), moral outrage (r = -.127), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = -.175)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in ascriptions and dishonesty in research and reporting (r = .514), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = .727)

· moderate, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in ascriptions and dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = 496)

· low, positive, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in ascriptions and cheating (r = 271)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between dishonesty in ascriptions and moral obligation (r = -.169), moral accountability (r = -.177), moral outrage (r = -.206), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = -. 203)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between deontic justice (combined score) and moral obligation (r = .920), moral accountability (r = .889), and moral outrage (r = .801)

· low, negative, statistically significant relationship between deontic justice (combined score) and tendency towards cheating (r = -.113), dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = -.111), dishonesty in research and reporting (r = -.175), dishonesty in ascriptions (r = -.203), and academic dishonesty (combined score) (r = -.191)

· strong, positive, and statistically significant relationship between academic dishonesty tendency (combined score) and tendency towards cheating (r = .722), dishonesty in assignments and projects (r = .842), dishonesty in research and reporting (r = .757), and dishonesty in ascriptions (r = .727)

· low, negative, and statistically significant relationship between academic dishonesty tendency (combined score) and moral obligation (r = -.171), moral accountabilty (r = -.167), moral outrage (r = .165), and deontic justice (combined score) (r = .191)

 

Discussion and Conclusion

The study reveals that there is a relationship between academic dishonesty tendency and deontic justice, which is an important finding implying that perceptions of justice affect students' behaviour in terms of dishonesty. It is essential to reduce dishonesty behaviour because academic dishonesty is globally becoming more common. Grimes' (2004) cross-cultural study on dishonesty in academics and business revealed that nearly three-fourths of college students in Eastern Europe were involved in some form of cheating. The situation in Turkey is similar to that of developed countries, where a study conducted on cheating behaviours of students attending the Education Faculty of Inonu University revealed that nearly 70% of students cheated in their exams (Akdağ & Günes, 2002). Another study conducted with students of the Faculty of Medicine at Fırat University found that 80% of the participants cheated at times (Semerci, 2004).

The motives for cheating are students' ambitions to achieve higher marks, the effort it takes to complete an assignment in a limited amount of time, a lack of self-confidence or self-control, lower academic motivation, and a tendency to violate rules (Oran, Can, Şenol & Hadımlı, 2016). As the study results show, another important factor is the perception of justice, which has an influence on cheating behaviour. Personal and socially constructed values, beliefs, and assumptions affect people's choices and understanding of what is morally right or wrong (Potgieter, 2011). In other words, choices and perceptions influence behaviour standards. This study reveals that there is a strong, positive and meaningful relationship between the combined score for deontic justice and sub-scales of moral obligation, moral accountability, and moral outrage. If a person feels obliged to behave morally, s/he feels deontic justice stronger. Transparency and accountability in terms of the moral issues are also important motives to feel deontic justice. If a person chooses to be morally responsible, s/he will behave accordingly, and as deontic justice implies, s/he will have the same feelings not only for her/himself, but also for others. There is a negative and meaningful relationship between combined the deontic justice score and the sub-scales of cheating, dishonesty in assignments, projects, dishonesty in research, reporting, and dishonesty in ascriptions. Although there is no strong relationship, it implies that, as the deontic justice score increases, dishonesty tendencies in academic studies decrease.

The findings also show that the academic dishonesty tendency combined score has a strong, positive, and meaningful relationship with its sub-scales of cheating, dishonesty in assignments, projects, dishonesty in research, reporting, and dishonesty in ascriptions. If an individual has a higher academic dishonesty tendency score, s/he more often acts dishonestly in academic studies. On the other hand, there is a low, negative, and meaningful relationship between academic dishonesty tendency and sub-scales of moral obligation, moral accountability, and moral outrage. Having a higher tendency towards academic dishonesty signals a lower moral obligation, moral accountability, and moral outrage scores. It can be said that if an individual has a tendency towards academic dishonesty, s/he doesn't a have strong feeling or a sense of responsibility to behave morally. The inverse relationship between combined scores of deontic justice and academic dishonesty tendency supports this view. Whitley (1998) found that students who have a negative attitude towards cheating do not cheat, even when they are less likely to get caught. In other words, students who do not find cheating moral, do not cheat, even if they have the opportunity to do so. At this point, moral obligation, accountability, and outrage gain importance. If students have a positive attitude towards deontic justice, they are less likely to exhibit the tendency towards academic dishonesty. In this respect, the study leads educators to have an impact on students' juctice perception. If students are motivated to adopt the view of justice for all, less academic dishonesty behaviour may be observed.

This study also indicates that male students are more prone to cheat, or to display academic dishonesty. The finding supports the results of the study conducted by Küçüktepe and Küçüktepe (2012), which investigated the tendency of prospective teachers of history towards academic dishonesty, and found that male participants had a higher tendency to cheat than female teacher candidates. Yangin and Kahyaoğlu (2009) found similar results in terms of gender in their study on cheating tendencies among prospective teachers of Primary Education. Likewise, most of the academic dishonesty research done in the United States of America and other western countries revealed that male students are more inclined to cheat in the exams. This may possibly be explained by the fact that male students feel less guilty when they cheat (Gümüşgül, Üstün, Işik & Demirel, 2013). Likewise, in respect of the sub-scale, moral outrage, female students have significantly higher scores, which reveal that female students have more positive deontic justice attitudes than do male students. This finding aligns with the findings from a study conducted by Ekşi, Okan and Güner (2016), which revealed that females have higher scores than males in terms of deontic justice. It can be inferred that the attitude of females towards deontic justice lowers their tendency towards academic dishonesty when compared to males.

When considering deontic justice attitudes and tendencies towards academic dishonesty among participants from different departments of education, the findings revealed that students of the Physical Education and Sports Department are more inclined to show academic dishonesty. This finding aligns to that of a study conducted by Omur, Aydin and Argon (2014), which investigated the relationship between the fear of negative evaluation and tendency towards academic dishonesty. It was found that students of the Fine Arts Education Department also had a relatively higher level of academic dishonesty tendency. This finding was explained by referring to the aptitude tests that are taken as a part of the admissions examinations. This is also true with reference to the Physical Education and Sports Departments. It can be inferred that the students who attend departments requiring physical or artistic aptitude do not place as much importance on written exams. Aptitude tests are seen to be more crucial for their future careers, and they may rationalise the tendency towards dishonesty with this kind of thinking. On the other hand, students of the English Language Education Department had the highest scores on the Deontic Justice Scale. Futher research should possibly be undertaken to understand the reasons behind such a difference.

To reduce the prevalence of academic dishonesty, the importance of academic integrity is emphasised. Teacher educators intentionally prepare prospective teachers as knowledgeable, thinking practitioners who are sensitive to moral and ethical issues (Rusznyak, 2018; Uredi & Akbasli, 2015). Students should take part in the enforcement of academic integrity initiatives (Melgoza & Smith, 2008). Ethical values ought to be stressed, and ethical development of students ought to be supported (Pavela, 1999). The importance and value of honesty, integrity, and courage should be emphasised, and students ought to be discouraged from cheating (Hinton, 2004). Educators' positive and supportive attitudes may decrease the frequency of cheating. Students' deontic justice attitudes should be developed because deontic perspectives emphasise ethical and fair behaviour (Beugré, 2010). It should be emphasised that honesty is a crucial merit, and it ought to be preferred over personal interests. For this reason, educators' attitudes are important, and as it is believed that attitudes are not like personality traits (Nel, Müller, Hugo, Helldin, Bäckmann, Dwyer & Skarlind, 2011), they can be changed. The attitudes of students of education towards academic dishonesty is, therefore, especially important. When they become teachers, they are expected to inculcate moral values in their students. The education system, therefore, needs individuals with moral maturity, and this may be possible to a greater degree if educators help their students to internalise the moral standards embodied by a deontic justice attitude.

In closing. This study focused on deontic justice attitudes and academic dishonesty tendencies of prospective teachers. The results are limited with findings relating to 403 particpants attending an Education Faculty in Turkey. Future studies can be done in different regions with different participants or variables.

 

Authors' Contributions

Each author contributed equally to the manuscript in terms of data collection, statistical analyses, and conceptual framework. All authors reviewed the final manuscript.

 

Notes

i. Published under a Creative Commons Attribution Licence.

ii. DATES: Received: 2 November 2017; Revised: 7 December 2018; Accepted: 8 March 2019; Published: 31 August 2019.

 

References

Akdağ M & Günes H 2002. Kopya çekme davranışları ve kopya çekmeye ilişkin tutumlar [Cheating behaviors and attitudes towards cheating]. Kuram ve Uygulamada Eğitim Yönetimi [Educational Management in Theory and Practice], 31:330-343.         [ Links ]

Akın A, Sarıçam H, Kaya Ç, Akdeniz C, Gediksiz E, Toprak H & Yıldız B 2013. Deontik Adalet Ölçeği Türkçe Formu: Geçerlik ve güvenirlik çalışması. VI. [The Turkish version of the deontic justice scale: The validity and reliability study. VI]. In Ulusal Lisansüstü Eğitim Sempozyumu Bildiriler Kitapçığı, Sakarya Üniversitesi Yayınları [National Graduate Education Symposium Proceedings Booklet]. Sakarya, Turkey: Sakarya University Publications.         [ Links ]

Beugré CD 2010. Resistance to socialization into organizational corruption: A model of deontic justice. Journal of Business and Psychology, 25(3):533-541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-010-9176-3        [ Links ]

Beugré CD 2012. Development and validation of a deontic justice scale. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42(9):2163-2190. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00935.x        [ Links ]

Blankenship KL & Whitley BE 2000. Relation of general deviance to academic dishonesty. Ethics & Behavior, 10(1):1-12. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327019EB1001_1        [ Links ]

Burrus RT, McGoldrick KM & Schuhmann PW 2007. Self-reports of student cheating: Does a definition of cheating matter? The Journal of Economic Education, 38(1):3-16. https://doi.org/10.3200/JECE.38.1.3-17        [ Links ]

Cropanzano R, Stein J & Goldman BM 2007. Individual aesthetics-self-interest. In EH Kessler & JR Bailey (eds). Handbook of organizational and managerial wisdom. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.         [ Links ]

Cropanzano RS, Massaro S & Beckers WJ 2017. Deontic justice and organizational neuroscience. Journal of Business Ethics, 144(4):733-754 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3056-3        [ Links ]

Dawkins RL 2004. Attributes and statuses of college students associated with classroom cheating on a small-sized campus. College Student Journal, 38(1):116-129. Available at https://stemed.unm.edu/sites/all/docs/Attributes_Associated_Classroom_Cheating.pdf. Accessed 2 July 2019.         [ Links ]

Du Preez P & Simmonds S 2011. Understanding how we understand girls' voices on cultural and religious practices: Toward a curriculum for justice. South African Journal of Education, 31(3):322-332. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v31n3a536        [ Links ]

Ekşi H, Okan N & Güner H 2016. İç göç yaşayan ergenlerin deontik adalet anlayışı ve akran sapmalarının şiddete yönelik tutumlarını yordama düzeyi [Comprehension of deontic justice in adolescents who have moved domestically: Peer deviance as a predictor of attitudes toward violence]. Uluslararası Mülteci Hakları Derneği [Middle East Journal of Refugee Studies], 1(1):19-41. https://doi.org/10.12738/mejrs.2016.1.1.0002        [ Links ]

Eminoglu E 2008. Üniversite öğrencilerinin akademik sahtekârlık eğilimlerinin ölçülmesine yönelik bir ölçek geliştirme çalışması [A scale development study to measure academic dishonesty tendency of university students]. Master's thesis. Bolu, Turkey: Bolu Abant Izzet Baysal University.         [ Links ]

Eminoglu E & Nartgün Z 2009. Üniversite öğrencilerinin akademik sahtekarlık eğilimlerinin ölçülmesine yönelik bir ölçek geliştirme çalışması [A scale development study to measure academic dishonesty tendency of university students]. Uluslararası İnsan Bilimleri Dergisi, 6(1):215-240.         [ Links ]

Eraslan A 2011. Matematik öğretmeni adayları ve kopya: Hiç çekmedim desem yalan olur! [Prospective mathematics teachers and cheating: It is a lie if I say I have never cheated]. Eğitim ve Bilim [Education and Science], 36(160):52-64.         [ Links ]

Ersoy A & Özden M 2011. Öğretmen adaylarının ödevlerinde intihal yapmalarında öğretim elemanının rolüne ilişkin görüşleri [The views of teacher candidates regarding the role of instructor in plagiarizing from Internet in their assignments]. İlköğretim Online [Elementary Education Online], 10(2):608-619.         [ Links ]

Finn KV & Frone MR 2004. Academic performance and cheating: Moderating role of school identification and self-efficacy. The Journal of Educational Research, 97(3):115-121. https://doi.org/10.3200/JOER.97.3.115-121        [ Links ]

Folger R 2001. Fairness as deonance. In S Gilliland, D Steiner & D Skarlicki (eds). Theoretical and cultural perspectives on organizational justice. Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.         [ Links ]

Greene J 2013. Moral tribes: Emotion, reason, and the gap between us and them. New York, NY: Penguin Books.         [ Links ]

Gresley J, Wallace H, Hubb JM & Staats S 2009. Heroes don't cheat: An examination of academic dishonesty and students' views on why professors don't report cheating. Ethics and Behavior, 19(3):171-183. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508420802623716        [ Links ]

Grimes PW 2004. Dishonesty in academics and business: A cross-cultural evaluation of student attitudes. Journal of Business Ethics, 49(3):273-290. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:BUSI.0000017969.29461.30        [ Links ]

Gümüşgül O, Üstün UD, Işik U & Demirel DH 2013. Beden eğitimi ve spor yüksekokulu öğrencilerinin akademik sahtekarlık eğilim düzeylerinin değerlendirilmesi [Evaluation of academic dishonesty tendency of students studying at School of Physical Education and Sports]. Ankara Üniversitesi Spor Bilimleri Fakültesi Dergisi, 11(2):131-138. Available at https://dergipark.org.tr/download/article-file/601126. Accessed 26 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Hinton L 2004. Plagiarism: Learning from our challenges. Studies in Learning, Evaluation Innovation and Development, 1(1):37-46.         [ Links ]

Hopkins WG 2008. Research designs: Choosing and fine-tuning a design for your study. Sportscience, 12:12-21. Available at https://learning.uonbi.ac.ke/courses/GPR100/document/ResearchMethodology&LR&WMaterials-ResearchDesign/ResearchDesignsWGHopkins.pdf. Accessed 22 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Karim S & Ghavam E 2011. The relationship between self-control, self-effectiveness, academic performance and tendency towards academic cheating: A case report of a university survey in Iran. Malaysian Journal of Distance Education, 13(2):1-8. Available at http://mjde.usm.my/vol13_2_2011/mjde13_2_1.pdf. Accessed 22 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Küçüktepe SE & Küçüktepe C 2012. Tarih öğretmeni adaylarinin kopya çekme eğilimlerinin çeşitli değişkenler açisindan incelenmesi [Evaluation of cheating tendency of history teacher candidates in terms of various variables]. Hasan Ali Yücel Eğitim Fakültesi Dergisi, 17(1):115-125. Available at https://dergipark.org.tr/download/article-file/93149. Accessed 22 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Lau VP & Wong YY 2009. Direct and multiplicative effects of ethical dispositions and ethical climates on personal justice norms: A virtue ethics perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 90(2):279-294. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-009-0042-z        [ Links ]

Lerner MJ 1975. The justice motive in social behavior: Introduction. Journal of Social Issues, 31(3):1-19. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1975.tb00995.x        [ Links ]

Lin CHS & Wen LYM 2007. Academic dishonesty in higher education -a nationwide study in Taiwan. Higher Education, 54(1):85-97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10734-006-9047-z        [ Links ]

McCabe DL, Trevino LK & Butterfield KD 2001. Cheating in academic institutions: A decade of research. Ethics & Behavior, 11(3):219-232. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327019EB1103_2        [ Links ]

Melgoza P & Smith J 2008. Revitalizing an existing honor code program. Innovative Higher Education, 32(4):209-219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10755-007-9048-6        [ Links ]

Nel N, Müller H, Hugo A, Helldin R, Bäckmann Ö, Dwyer H & Skarlind A 2011. A comparative perspective on teacher attitude-constructs that impact on inclusive education in South Africa and Sweden. South African Journal of Education, 31(1):74-90. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v31n1a414        [ Links ]

Odabaşi HF, Birinci G, Kiliçer K, Şahin MC, Akbulut Y & Şendağ S 2007. Bilgi iletişim teknolojileri ve internet'le kolaylaşan akademik usulsüzlük [Academic dishonesty: Getting easier with Internet and ICT]. Sosyal Bilimler Dergisi, 1:503-518. Available at http://www.acarindex.com/dosyalar/makale/acarindex-1423869663.pdf. Accessed 13 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Omur YE, Aydin R & Argon T 2014. Relationship between prospective teachers' fear of negative evaluation and their academic dishonesty tendencies. Journal of Education and Humanities: Theory and Practice, 5(9):131-149.         [ Links ]

O'Neill HM & Pfeiffer CA 2012. The impact of honour codes and perceptions of cheating on academic cheating behaviours, especially for MBA bound undergraduates. Accounting Education, 21(3):231-245. https://doi.org/10.1080/09639284.2011.590012        [ Links ]

Oran NT, Can HÖ, Şenol S & Hadımlı AP 2016. Academic dishonesty among health science school students. Nursing Ethics, 23(8):919-931. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0969733015583929        [ Links ]

Orosz G, Dombi E, Tóth-Király I, Bőthe B, Jagodics B & Zimbardo PG 2016. Academic cheating and time perspective: Cheaters live in the present instead of the future. Learning and Individual Differences, 52:39-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.007        [ Links ]

Pallant J 2010. SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using the SPSS program (4th ed). Maidenhead, England: Open University Press.         [ Links ]

Pavela G 1999. A renewed focus on student ethical development. Synthesis: Law and Policy in Higher Education, 10(3):733.         [ Links ]

Potgieter FJ 2011. Morality as the substructure of social justice: Religion in education as a case in point. South African Journal of Education, 31(3):394-406. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v31n3a537        [ Links ]

Quick G & Siebörger R 2005. What matters in practice teaching? The perceptions of schools and students. South African Journal of Education, 25(1):1-4.         [ Links ]

Rusznyak L 2018. What messages about teacher professionalism are transmitted through South African pre-service teacher education programmes? South African Journal of Education, 38(3):Art. #1503, 11 pages. https://doi.org/10.15700/saje.v38n3a1503        [ Links ]

Semerci C 2004. Tip fakültesi öğrencilerinin kopya çekmeye ilişkin tutum ve görüşleri [Attitudes and opinions towards cheating of medicine faculty students]. Fırat Üniversitesi Sağlık Bilimleri Dergisi [Firat University Journal of Health Sciences], 18(3):139-146. Available at http://tip.fusabil.org/pdf/pdf_FUSABIL_335.pdf. Accessed 6 June 2019.         [ Links ]

Skarlicki DP & Kulik C 2005. Third party reactions to employee mistreatment: A justice perspective. In B Staw & R Kramer (eds). Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 26). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.         [ Links ]

Tsui APY & Ngo HY 2016. Social-motivational factors affecting business students' cheating behavior in Hong Kong and China. Journal of Education for Business, 91(7):365-373. https://doi.org/10.1080/08832323.2016.1231108        [ Links ]

Uredi L & Akbasli S 2015. Classroom teachers' self-efficacy beliefs on constructivist approach. Anthropologist, 20(1,2):268-279. Available at http://krepublishers.com/02-Journals/T-Anth/Anth-20-0-000-15-Web/Anth-20-1-000-15-Abst-PDF/T-ANTH-20-1,2-268-15-1441-Uredi-Akbas-I/T-ANTH-20-1,2-268-15-1441-Uredi-Akbas-I-Tx[29].pdf. Accessed 23 August 2019.         [ Links ]

Whitley BE 1998. Factors associated with cheating among college students: A review. Research in Higher Education, 39(3):235-274. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018724900565        [ Links ]

Yangin S & Kahyaoğlu M 2009. İlköğretim öğretmen adaylarinin kopya çekmeye yönelik tutum ve görüşleri [Attitudes and ideas of candidate primary school teachers towards copying]. Balıkesir Üniversitesi Sosyal Bilimler Enstitüsü Dergisi, 12(21):46-55. Available at http://sbe.balikesir.edu.tr/dergi/edergi/c12s21/makale/c12s21m5.pdf. Accessed 8 May 2019.         [ Links ]

Yazıcıoğlu Y & Erdoğan S 2004. SPSS uygulamalı bilimsel araştırma yöntemleri [Scientific research techniques with SPSS]. Ankara, Turkey: Detay Yayıncılık.         [ Links ]

Creative Commons License All the contents of this journal, except where otherwise noted, is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License