SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.114 número5-6Fires, science and societyFrederik van Zyl Slabbert (1940-2010) índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


South African Journal of Science

versión On-line ISSN 1996-7489
versión impresa ISSN 0038-2353

S. Afr. j. sci. vol.114 no.5-6 Pretoria may./jun. 2018

http://dx.doi.org/10.17159/sajs.2018/20170321 

REVIEW ARTICLE

 

Trends in behavioural ecology: Putting South African research in a global perspective

 

 

Aliza le Roux

Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of the Free State, Qwaqwa Campus, Phuthaditjhaba, South Africa

Correspondence

 

 


ABSTRACT

South Africa remains at the leading edge of scientific publishing on the African continent, yet few analyses of publication patterns exist outside the biomedical field. Considering the large number of protected areas and mammalian guilds within the country, I examined trends in South African ecological research as it pertains to the behaviour of mammals. I assessed the topics and taxonomic focus of mammalogists at South African institutes over the span of 15 years (2001-2015), and contrasted local research with the shifting focus of international behavioural research. This review of more than 1000 publications indicates that South African based researchers exhibit a strong tendency towards field-based research, as opposed to laboratory-centred experiments. In terms of topical focus, local ecologists place significant weight on the behavioural categories of mating, social and foraging behaviour - reflecting a global priority for these topics. This finding contrasts with an increased emphasis on animal cognition and communication research in the international research arena, including field-based studies on these themes. I make suggestions on how behavioural ecologists in South Africa can align themselves with global trends while also continuing to distinguish those facets that make South African behavioural ecology unique.
SIGNIFICANCE:
This review is the first of behavioural ecology in South Africa.
Suggestions are made for where South African researchers can profitably shift research focus.
International trends in behavioural ecology are highlighted.

Keywords: animal behaviour; bibliometrics; Mammalia; research impact; ecology


 

 

Introduction

South Africa remains at the forefront of scientific publishing on the African continent, with Life Sciences contributing strongly to these research outputs.1 Within this broader discipline, we also have the relatively unique advantage of direct access to intact mammalian guilds2 inside well-managed protected areas3 - a factor that has stimulated significant mammalogical research in the country. However, no broad meta-analysis of local publishing patterns currently exists, and by some accounts few South African researchers are investigating mammalian behaviour: within the Zoological Society of Southern Africa, the Ethology research group was short-lived4, and in the past 15 years only one paper on mammalian behaviour has been published in South Africa's top multidisciplinary journal, the South African Journal of Science5. This is surprising, as evolutionary biology is thriving in South Africa6 and behavioural ecology is by definition the integration of evolutionary biology with the observation of animal behaviour. Since 19637, when Tinbergen created his seminal framework for studying behaviour (traditional 'ethology') in an evolutionary context, the study of behavioural ecology has thrived internationally8. How well, then, is South African research represented within this field?

I reviewed the study of mammalian behaviour in South Africa, describing trends in behavioural ecology over the course of 15 years (2001-2015) and highlighting the evolution of this discipline. As a framework, I use the 10 themes of behavioural ecology recently developed by Berger-Tal and colleagues9 as an accurate reflection on the specialities within this research field. I discuss specific strengths and weaknesses of behavioural ecology research in South Africa and indicate opportunities for continued growth.

 

Methodology

I conducted an extensive survey of international literature on mammalian behavioural ecology using the SCOPUS database. Search terms were deliberately broad to encompass all literature (articles/review papers) from 2001 to 2015 that included the term "behav*" and "mammal*" in the title, abstract or keywords. In addition, I constructed a list of all mammalian families, thereby creating a search phrase to check if any of the mammalian families were included in the title, abstract or keywords of the publication. An initial search specifying these broad terms yielded

11 330 articles and review papers. This figure is an indication of the number of publications that describe behaviour, but is likely an exaggeration of the research that deals specifically and exclusively with mammalian behaviour.

I refined my search to compare South African research with the international literature, and created two publication databases. In the first, I specified that the author affiliation had to include "South Africa". These authors may have had multiple affiliations, and affiliation with a South African institute did not necessarily imply this was their primary place of work. I compiled a second database of international research that explicitly excluded authors with South African affiliations. In the second database, I limited the search to journals that focus specifically on animal behaviour: Behavioral Ecology (impact factor (IF)=3.177, from http://www.citefactor.org/ accessed on 24 November 2016), Animal Behaviour (IF=3.137), Applied Animal Behaviour Science (IF=1.691), Advances in the Study of Behaviour (IF=2.692), and Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (IF=2.35). Furthermore, I included publications on mammalian behaviour that featured in the top three multidisciplinary journals, to ensure that I did not ignore research with the putative highest impact. These journals were: Science (2013 IF=31.477), Nature (2013 IF=42.351), and Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (2013 IF=9.423). Once the articles were extracted from the SCOPUS database, I analysed all abstracts to exclude papers that did not focus on, or explicitly include mammalian behavioural ecology. This process produced a total of 371 South African papers and 901 international publications. This refined selection of South African papers suggests that at least 3.3% of all articles on mammalian behaviour include South African affiliations (371 out of 11 330). Importantly, as I was selective in the outlets for international publications in particular, the latter database did not constitute all international publications on mammalian behaviour - it is a reflection of the articles appearing in the more respected and subject-specific journals, and therefore indicative of broader trends in the field.

Within these databases, I examined the taxonomic breadth of study species as well as the focus on different techniques or approaches in the local and international literature. To aid the thematic analyses, I performed text mining to identify the most common keywords, using the 'tm' package10 in RStudio for Mac (version 0.99.903). Finally, I pooled the papers for each database into three 5-year blocks: (1) 2001-2005; (2) 2006-2010; and (3) 2011-2015, and used the 10 behavioural themes identified in a recent review9 to guide my assessment of changing trends.

 

Findings and discussion

Overview: South African contributions

South African researchers contributed to >3% of the global literature on mammalian behavioural ecology, which is higher than the 1% contribution that sub-Saharan African countries make to global scientific research in general.11 As the original database of articles on mammalian behaviour was likely an exaggerated number (I did not go through all 11 330 abstracts to ascertain each study's focus), the true proportion of South African affiliations will be even higher. The majority of South African papers were primary research articles, with local authors publishing review papers at a somewhat higher rate (2.5% of the South African database) than international authors (1.5% of the international database). While it is positive to see this contribution to the synthesis of the field, South African ecologists appear to have had a smaller impact than international scientists: only 13.2% of our research has been cited more than five times per year, in contrast to nearly one third (31.7%) of international articles being cited at the same rate. The South African ecologists who publish in top multidisciplinary journals (e.g. Slotow et al.12) often base their research on long-term field data, that is, studies that continue beyond a three-year project.

While many local and international publications claimed to investigate behaviour in an evolutionary context, South African researchers were more likely to address the ultimate causes and consequences of behaviour (in contrast to proximate or mechanistic causes of behaviour). South African authors tend to study species within the broader ecological context, incorporating aspects of the habitat13, ecosystem14, and the survival/fitness value15 of different behaviours. Population dynamics and population-level analyses were common in South African research, often including genetic analyses of population structure.16 In one third (36.4%) of South African papers17,18, authors linked behaviour to conservation, compared with a much more limited focus on conservation in the international literature (1.8% of the literature, e.g. Palphramand et al.19). South African researchers therefore appear to align themselves well with calls for the integration of behaviour and conservation.9 Furthermore, many South African researchers conducted experiments in their behavioural research, with nearly half (46.1%) of experiment-driven articles based on field studies.20,21

By contrast, in the international literature, 83.7% of experiments took place in captive or laboratory-based settings22, and a further 5.4% of these experiments utilised domestic or farmed animals23. International scientists often followed a mechanistic approach to the analysis of mammalian behaviour, with a large proportion of publications investigating causes and consequences of behaviour on a cellular level.24 These authors also frequently studied the mechanisms of circadian rhythms25 and echolocation26, which are largely absent from the South African database. International researchers exhibited a high interest in animal welfare research27, likely reflecting the prevalence of captive species in their taxonomic repertoire.

The most popular taxonomic clade in both databases was Rodentia; however, international studies concentrated on laboratory rodents28, contrasting with a much broader focus in South African research (including mole rats14 and Rhabdomys29species). Carnivores were also prominent study subjects, with meerkats (Suricata suricatta) dominating both databases.30,31 Internationally, canids32 and spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta33) featured strongly in behavioural research, whereas South African researchers more often investigated Felidae34. Antelope35and small carnivores outside the Herpestidae family36 were amongst the least studied taxa.

Dominant themes: Foraging, reproduction and social behaviour

Research in most thematic areas increased over the review period (Table 1), signifying a growing trend for researchers to address multiple themes within one study. South African and international authors largely agreed in terms of the top three thematic areas: foraging, reproductive and social behaviour (Table 1, Figure 1). In contrast to international trends, in which indirect analysis of foraging behaviour was rarely represented37, many South African researchers use indirect methods such as scat38and isotope analyses39 to describe foraging behaviour. Foraging ecology in the South African literature is therefore often restricted to dietary studies, with nuanced assessments of foraging behaviour - such as habitat selection13 or strategic responses to environmental variation40 -remaining uncommon.

Locally and internationally, research on mating and reproductive behaviour often assesses the hormonal correlates of reproductive success and/or helping behaviour. While common in the international literature41, the endocrinology of reproduction is particularly widespread in the South African database (for example see Marneweck et al.15). In both databases, much of the research focuses on reproductive control or suppression42, including rarely documented abortion43.

Within the theme of social behaviour, relatively few researchers examined social behaviour and social structure as a goal in itself. Sociality was nearly always linked to other aspects of an animal's behavioural ecology, such as anti-predator and foraging behaviour44, or competition45. The social behaviour of solitary species or solitary foragers was also examined fairly often by both local46 and international authors47. The progression of this theme, beyond the basic description of social systems in gregarious species, is likely a reflection of the maturity of sociobiology as a research discipline.

Discrepancies: Communication, cognition and behavioural syndromes

A sharp contrast emerged between South African and international literature in terms of the next two high-priority themes. Internationally, communication and cognition were topics that featured in 17.7% and 16.1% of all articles, respectively. Comparatively speaking, South African authors largely have ignored these themes in the past 15 years, with only 2.7% of papers focused on learning and cognition, and 6.5% of publications investigating mammalian communication. The few South African publications examining mammalian cognition follow international trends in terms of topic, highlighting innovative behaviour in wild mammals48, and exploring the link between stress and cognition49. One unusual study concluded that large brain size in dolphins was driven less by cognitive demands, than by low water temperatures.50 This South African study stands in sharp contrast to seminal international articles linking brain size to sociality51 and - more rarely - the demands of complex foraging52. Within the theme of mammalian communication, South Africans agree with international research in preferentially studying acoustic signals53, above other communication modalities54.

Over the review period, the theme of animal personality, or behavioural syndromes, was not investigated even once by South African mammalogists. The bulk of international publications (n=14) on the topic of personality appeared after 2010, although no papers on the theme appeared in the multidisciplinary journals Science or Nature. Mammalian personality or temperament is most often examined in the context of stress response55, survival56 and fitness57.

The discrepancies between South African and international trends are less severe in the remaining five themes, and both databases present a similar distribution of topics. There are a few subtle distinctions in focus area. In terms of competitive behaviour, South African authors examine interspecific interactions58 more often than international scientists do59. Within the theme of anti-predator behaviour, it is interesting to note that only international researchers appeared to consider the consequences of humans and human infrastructure for anti-predator behaviour60, while vigilance61 remains a common focus across both databases. I could find no remarkable distinctions between South African and international researchers in the study of mammalian migration and dispersal. Finally, in terms of parental care, the primary focus of both data sets was on maternal care, with South Africans48 assessing paternal care more often than international authors62.

 

The way forward

In a very practical sense, South African mammalogists are making optimal use of the accessible 'walk-in' laboratory available in the country's conserved areas and relatively intact ecosystems. A prominent focus on conservation-related research suggests that these researchers are cognisant of the risk of losing these fauna, and are actively directing projects to meet the challenges of a human-dominated global landscape. It is important to note, however, that using 'conservation' as a key term in research does not imply that practical solutions to conservation-related problems are presented. Indeed, other reviewers9 have pointed out that researchers who wish to have a positive impact on conservation challenges need to consciously conduct research that proposes, examines and practically explains answers to conservation-related questions. This is unfortunately not currently the norm in South African (or international) conservation biology.

Whereas behavioural ecology in South Africa is currently on firm footing, local researchers' impact can be more profound if we consciously adopt a more integrated approach. In a recent review, Bateson and Laland8highlighted that behavioural ecologists tend to ask specific behavioural questions in isolation, rarely combining a proximate and ultimate angle within a single study. This challenge remains despite the fact that Tinbergen7 advocated for integration over 50 years ago, and South African researchers appear to be as guilty of this narrow theoretical scope as international researchers are. We could start addressing this challenge by building on our strengths. In the South African literature, the most cited research often addresses movement and foraging ecology (predation, in particular), and we have a strong national focus on studying carnivores. We could - and should - move beyond the study of movement and foraging behaviour (see also Young and Shivik63), building on the unique strengths of this mammalian order. For example, carnivores exhibit paternal care - a rarity amongst mammals64 - more frequently than other mammals, yet we know almost nothing about the physiological drivers and consequences of this behaviour65. New studies should examine movement and foraging behaviour from both a proximate and ultimate angle, assessing the physiological covariates of dispersal and breeding behaviour, as well as the longer-term consequences of these individual decisions for fitness and population dynamics. Furthermore, while South African researchers already display a positive tendency to study heterospecific interactions, these studies are often performed on either a population level (e.g. Codron et al.39), or much smaller scale58. It is likely that collaborative research across study sites would enable us to assess inter-specific competition and collaboration on both a fine and large scale.

South African researchers appear slow to respond to some changing international trends that could open up new research and funding avenues. Specifically, local mammalogists pay scant attention to the themes of mammalian communication and cognition, contrasting with international trends. Importantly, international interest in the topic of animal cognition is far higher than this review would suggest, considering that I excluded subject-specific journals such as Animal Cognition (IF=1.122) from the review. Further, studies on mammalian communication and cognition are prominent amongst the most-cited papers in the international literature. Animal communication and cognition are, in many ways, closely linked. Not only is communicative complexity deemed indicative of cognitive complexity66, but researchers often use animal signals in experiments designed to examine particular aspects of animal cognition67. South African authors could benefit from looking beyond the topics that have sustained local behavioural ecology for a long time, and steer some research focus into the minds and communicative abilities of our rich mammalian fauna. Of particular interest would be linking these themes with conservation biology.68 For example, basic associative learning experiments can be used to train naïve animals to avoid predators69 or avoid novel, toxic prey species70. Knowledge of how prey animals detect predators using olfactory, visual or acoustic cues could potentially improve the management of direct human-wildlife conflict: acoustic deterrents have been used to reduce crop raiding by elephants71, and prey avoidance of predator faeces72suggests that such chemical cues can be used to deter some pests.

Another significant difference between South African and international research is that international scientists place a big emphasis on mechanistic studies of behaviour, often in captive study populations. This is particularly true for the behavioural research showcased in the top multidisciplinary journals, Nature and Science. Although this focus is likely a pragmatic response to the local paucity of wildlife, such controlled conditions open up novel research opportunities. The study of animal personality, for example, is largely rooted in laboratory or captive studies, and has only recently become more mainstream as a topic that has serious implications for animal survival and evolution.73There are endless possibilities for investigating the unique physiology and behaviour of our local fauna, particularly smaller species that may more readily adapt to captive environments. Already, some novel South African led research has emerged from research into captive mammals, such as an examination of how rising environmental temperatures impact sleep behaviour in bats (Epomophorus wahlbergi74). Certainly, local researchers could also tap into the international fascination with the mammalian brain, potentially through active collaboration with medical researchers who have access to high-end scanning equipment.

South African researchers' successful scientific exploitation of our abundant natural resources may have inadvertently created a research blindspot in that we literally do not look closer to home to investigate the myriad ways in which mammals respond to humans and human infrastructure. With humans often living inside or adjacent to protected areas in South Africa, and tourism being a primary source of national income, studying the direct and indirect interaction between humans or human infrastructure and wildlife is becoming imperative. At the moment, South African researchers have not looked much further than crop raiding or the negative impacts of primates 'invading' human-modified areas.75,76 Thus, we typically concentrate on the negative impacts of human-wildlife on humans and largely ignore the impact of humans on the behaviour of local wildlife. Directions of research could include describing the behaviour of various species in urban areas77, or assessing responses to humans as tourists78, road users60 and scientific observers79.

South African behavioural ecologists would undeniably benefit from more collaboration between institutions, locations and fields of expertise. We still tend to work in silos (the majority of South African articles stemmed from the Mammal Research Institute of the University of Pretoria), and even concentrate on specific geographical locations, ignoring nature reserves that do not form part of the 'Big Five' national parks.80 Funding bodies should also consider encouraging long-term field projects, which generate insights into the evolution of behaviour that no short-term approach could yield. Simultaneously, South African researchers should not ignore the possibilities inherent in well-designed laboratory and captive research that enable the detailed assessment of proximate corollaries of animal behaviour. We will not, however, make any great leaps forward without conversation. The time may be right for the revival of the Ethology research group as part of the Zoological Society of Southern Africa.

 

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to the University of the Free State's Office of the Research Directorate for research leave that enabled me to write this paper. I would also like to thank Diana Breshears for invaluable feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript.

 

References

1. Pouris A. A bibliometric assessment of energy research in South Africa. S Afr J Sci. 2016;112:69-76. https://doi.org/10.17159/sajs.2016/20160054        [ Links ]

2. Cameron EZ, Du Toit JT. Winning by a neck: Tall giraffes avoid competing with shorter browsers. Am Nat. 2007;169:130-135. https://doi.org/10.1086/509940        [ Links ]

3. Licht DS, Kenner BC, Roddy DE. A comparison of the South African and United States models of natural areas management. ISRN Biodivers. 2014;2014:1-7. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/737832        [ Links ]

4. Taylor P Hamer M. Standing on the shoulders of colourful giants: 50 years of zoological research in southern Africa. Afr Zool. 2009;44:217-231. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2009.11407455        [ Links ]

5. Midgley JJ, Balfour D, Kerley GI. Why do elephants damage savanna trees? S Afr J Sci. 2005;101:213-215.         [ Links ]

6. Johnson SD. Darwin' s legacy in South African evolutionary biology. S Afr J Sci. 2009;105:403-409.         [ Links ]

7. Tinbergen N. On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol. 1963;20:410-433. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x        [ Links ]

8. Bateson P Laland KN. Tinbergen's four questions: An appreciation and an update. Trends Ecol Evol. 2013;28:712-718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.09.013        [ Links ]

9. Berger-Tal O, Blumstein DT, Carroll S, Fisher RN, Mesnick SL, Owen MA, et al. A systematic survey of the integration of animal behavior into conservation. Conserv Biol. 2016;30:744-753. https://doi.org/10.1111/cobi.12654        [ Links ]

10. Feinerer I, Hornik K, Meyer D. Text mining infrastructure in R. J Stat Softw. 2008;25:1-54. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v025.i05        [ Links ]

11. Tijssen RJW. Africa's contribution to the worldwide research literature: New analytical perspectives, trends, and performance indicators. Scientometrics. 2007;71:303-327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-007-1658-3        [ Links ]

12. Slotow R, Van Dyk G, Poole J, Page B, Klocke A. Older bull elephants control young males. Nature. 2000;408:425-426. https://doi.org/10.1038/35044191        [ Links ]

13. Somers MJ, Nel JAJ. Habitat selection by the Cape clawless otter (Aonyx capensis) in rivers in the Western Cape Province, South Africa. Afr J Ecol. 2004;42:298-305. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2028.2004.00526.x        [ Links ]

14. Hagenah N, Bennett NC. Mole rats act as ecosystem engineers within a biodiversity hotspot, the Cape Fynbos. J Zool. 2013;289:19-26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00958.x        [ Links ]

15. Marneweck D, Cameron EZ, Ganswindt A, Dalerum F. Behavioural and endocrine correlates to the aardwolf mating system. Mamm Biol. 2015;80:31-38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mambio.2014.08.001        [ Links ]

16. Fontaine MC, Baird SJ, Piry S, Ray N, Tolley KA, Duke S, et al. Rise of oceanographic barriers in continuous populations of a cetacean: The genetic structure of harbour porpoises in Old World waters. BMC Biol. 2007;5:30. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-5-30        [ Links ]

17. Hucke-Gaete R, Osman LP, Moreno CA, Findlay KP, Ljungblad DK. Discovery of a blue whale feeding and nursing ground in southern Chile. Proc R Soc London B Biol Sci. 2004;271(suppl 4):S170-173. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0132        [ Links ]

18. McMahon CR, Bester MN, Hindell MA, Brook BW, Bradshaw CJ. Shifting trends: Detecting environmentally mediated regulation in long-lived marine vertebrates using time-series data. Oecologia. 2009;59:69-82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-008-1205-9        [ Links ]

19. Palphramand KL, Newton-Cross G, White PCL. Spatial organization and behaviour of badgers (Meies meles) in a moderate-density population. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;61:401-413. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0268-z        [ Links ]

20. Dufour CMS, Meynard C, Watson J, Rioux C, Benhamou S, Perez J, et al. Space use variation in co-occurring sister species: Response to environmental variation or competition? PLoS ONE. 2015;10, e0117750, 15 pages. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117750        [ Links ]

21. Jackson C, Bernard RTF. Effects of supplementary food on the winter inhibition of reproduction in male and female four-striped field mice (Rhabdomys pumilio). Reprod Fertil Dev. 2005;17:393. https://doi.org/10.1071/RD04134        [ Links ]

22. Charlton BD, Ellis WAH, Brumm J, Nilsson K, Fitch WT. Female koalas prefer bellows in which lower formants indicate larger males. Anim Behav. 2012;84:1565-1571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.09.034        [ Links ]

23. Tallet C, Veissier I, Boivin X. A note on the consistency and specificity of lambs' responses to a stockperson and to their photograph in an arena test. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2006;98:308-314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.09.005        [ Links ]

24. Dosmann A, Brooks KC, Mateo JM. Evidence for a mechanism of phenotypic integration of behaviour and innate immunity in a wild rodent: Implications for animal personality and ecological immunology. Anim Behav. 2015;101:179-189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.12.026        [ Links ]

25. Maywood ES, Chesham JE, O'Brien JA, Hastings MH. A diversity of paracrine signals sustains molecular circadian cycling in suprachiasmatic nucleus circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:14306-14311. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1101767108        [ Links ]

26. Voigt-Heucke SL, Taborsky M, Dechmann DKN. A dual function of echolocation: Bats use echolocation calls to identify familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Anim Behav. 2010;80:59-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.03.025        [ Links ]

27. Olsson IAS, Westlund K. More than numbers matter: The effect of social factors on behaviour and welfare of laboratory rodents and nonhuman primates. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007;103:229-254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.022        [ Links ]

28. Roberts SA, Davidson AJ, Beynon RJ, Hurst JL. Female attraction to male scent and associative learning: The house mouse as a mammalian model. Anim Behav. 2014;97:313-321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.08.010        [ Links ]

29. Raynaud J, Müller K, Schradin C. Experimental increase of testosterone levels in free-ranging juvenile male African striped mice (Rhabdomys pumilio) induces physiological, morphological, and behavioral changes. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2012;178:108-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.04.028        [ Links ]

30. MacLeod KJ, Clutton-Brock TH. Low costs of allonursing in meerkats: Mitigation by behavioral change? Behav Ecol. 2015;26:697-705. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru205        [ Links ]

31. Graw B, Manser MB. The function of mobbing in cooperative meerkats. Anim Behav. 2007;74:507-517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.021        [ Links ]

32. Mettler AE, Shivik JA. Dominance and neophobia in coyote (Canis latrans) breeding pairs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2007;102:85-94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.03.012        [ Links ]

33. Curren LJ, Linden DW, Heinen VK, McGuire MC, Holekamp KE. The functions of male-male aggression in a female-dominated mammalian society. Anim Behav. 2015;100:208-216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.024        [ Links ]

34. Hayward M, O'Brien J, Kerley G. Carrying capacity of large African predators: Predictions and tests. Biol Conserv. 2007;139:219-229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2007.06.018        [ Links ]

35. Wronski T, Apio A, Plath M. The communicatory significance of localised defecation sites in bushbuck (Tragelaphus scriptus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:368-378. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0174-4        [ Links ]

36. Damasceno J, Genaro G. Dynamics of the access of captive domestic cats to a feed environmental enrichment item. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;151:67-74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.11.004        [ Links ]

37. Christianson D, Creel S. Risk effects in elk: Sex-specific responses in grazing and browsing due to predation risk from wolves. Behav Ecol. 2008;19:1258- 1266. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn079        [ Links ]

38. Ramesh T, Downs CT. Diet of serval (Leptailurus serval) on farmlands in the Drakensberg Midlands, South Africa. Mammalia. 2015;79:399-407. https://doi.org/10.1515/mammalia-2014-0053        [ Links ]

39. Codron J, Duffy KJ, Avenant NL, Sponheimer M, Leichliter J, Paine O, et al. Stable isotope evidence for trophic niche partitioning in a South African savanna rodent community. Curr Zool 2015;61:397-411. https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/61.3.397        [ Links ]

40. Woolley L-A, Millspaugh JJ, Woods RJ, Van Rensburg SJ, Page BR, Slotow R. Intraspecific strategic responses of African elephants to temporal variation in forage quality. J Wildl Manage. 2009;73:827-835. https://doi.org/10.2193/2008-412        [ Links ]

41. Nguyen N, Gesquiere LR, Wango EO, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Late pregnancy glucocorticoid levels predict responsiveness in wild baboon mothers (Papio cynocephalus). Anim Behav. 2008;75:1747-1756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.035        [ Links ]

42. Faulkes CG, Bennett NC. Family values: Group dynamics and social control of reproduction in African mole-rats. Trends Ecol Evol. 2001;16:184-190. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(01)02116-4        [ Links ]

43. Roberts EK, Lu A, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC. A Bruce effect in wild geladas. Science. 2012;335:1222-1225. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1213600        [ Links ]

44. Shrader AM, Post JF, Hagenah N, Bateman PW. Is a reduction in the individual vigilance of mothers a key evolutionary driver of group formation in white rhinos? Afr Zool. 2013;48:109-114. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2013.11407573        [ Links ]

45. Hodge SJ, Flower TP, Clutton-Brock TH. Offspring competition and helper associations in cooperative meerkats. Anim Behav. 2007;74:957-964. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.029        [ Links ]

46. Bray TC, Bloomer P O'Riain MJ, Bennett NC. How attractive is the girl next door? An assessment of spatial mate acquisition and paternity in the solitary Cape dune mole-rat, Bathyergus suillus. PLoS ONE. 2012;7, e39866, 8 pages. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0039866        [ Links ]

47. Tchabovsky AV Popov SV Krasnov BR. Intra- and interspecific variation in vigilance and foraging of two gerbillid rodents, Rhombomys opimus and Psammomys obesus: The effect of social environment. Anim Behav. 2001;62:965-972. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1833        [ Links ]

48. Le Roux A, Beishuizen R, Brekelmans W, Ganswindt A, Paris M, Dalerum F. Innovative parental care in a myrmecophageous mammal. Acta Ethol. 2014;17:63-66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-013-0157-1        [ Links ]

49. De Vos A, O'Riain JM, Meyer MA, Kotze PG, Kock AA. Behavior of Cape fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus pusillus) in relation to temporal variation in predation risk by white sharks (Carcharodon carcharías) around a seal rookery in False Bay, South Africa. Mar Mammal Sci. 2015;31:1118-1131. https://doi.org/10.1111/mms.12208        [ Links ]

50. Manger PR. An examination of cetacean brain structure with a novel hypothesis correlating thermogenesis to the evolution of a big brain. Biol Rev. 2006;81:293. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793106007019        [ Links ]

51. Reader SM, Laland KN. Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:4436-4441. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.062041299        [ Links ]

52. McLean AN. Cognitive abilities -- the result of selective pressures on food acquisition? Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001;71:241-258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(00)00181-7        [ Links ]

53. Kriesell HJ, Elwen SH, Nastasi A, Gridley T. Identification and characteristics of signature whistles in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) from Namibia. PLoS ONE. 2014;9, e106317, 13 pages. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106317        [ Links ]

54. Jordan NR, Cherry MI, Manser MB. Latrine distribution and patterns of use by wild meerkats: Implications for territory and mate defence. Anim Behav. 2007;73:613-622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.06.010        [ Links ]

55. Ferrari C, Pasquaretta C, Carere C, Cavallone E, Von Hardenberg A, Réale D. Testing for the presence of coping styles in a wild mammal. Anim Behav. 2013;85:1385-1396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.030        [ Links ]

56. Rödel HG, Zapka M, Talke S, Kornatz T, Bruchner B, Hedler C. Survival costs of fast exploration during juvenile life in a small mammal. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2015;69:205-217. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-014-1833-5        [ Links ]

57. Rangassamy M, Dalmas M, Féron C, Gouat P Rödel HG. Similarity of personalities speeds up reproduction in pairs of a monogamous rodent. Anim Behav. 2015;103:7-15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.007        [ Links ]

58. Do Linh San E, Somers MJ. Mongooses on the move: An apparent case of interspecific cooperative vigilance between carnivores. S Afr J Wildl Res. 2006;36:201-203.         [ Links ]

59. Wauters L, Gurnell J, Martinoli A, Tosi G. Interspecific competition between native Eurasian red squirrels and alien grey squirrels: Does resource partitioning occur? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2002;52:332-341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-002-0516-9        [ Links ]

60. Shannon G, Angeloni LM, Wittemyer G, Fristrup KM, Crooks KR. Road traffic noise modifies behaviour of a keystone species. Anim Behav. 2014;94:135-141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.06.004        [ Links ]

61. Schmitt MH, Stears K, Wilmers CC, Shrader AM. Determining the relative importance of dilution and detection for zebra foraging in mixed-species herds. Anim Behav. 2014;96:151-158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.08.012        [ Links ]

62. Buchan JC, Alberts SC, Silk JB, Altmann J. True paternal care in a multi-male primate society. Nature. 2003;425:179-181. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature01866        [ Links ]

63. Young JK, Shivik JA. What carnivore biologists can learn from bugs, birds, and beavers: A review of spatial theories. Can J Zool. 2006;84:1703-1711. https://doi.org/10.1139/z06-178        [ Links ]

64. Kleiman DG, Malcolm JR. The evolution of male parental investment in mammals. In: Gubernick DJ, Klopfer PH, editors. Parental care in mammals. New York: Plenum Publishing Corporation; 1981. p. 347-386. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-3150-6_9        [ Links ]

65. De Bruin R, Ganswindt A, Le Roux A. From killer to carer: Steroid hormones and paternal behaviour. Afr Zool. 2016;51:173-182. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2016.1258327        [ Links ]

66. Kaplan G. Animal communication. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2014;5:661-677. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1321        [ Links ]

67. Frommolt K-H, Goltsman ME, Macdonald DW. Barking foxes, Alopexlagopus: Field experiments in individual recognition in a territorial mammal. Anim Behav. 2003;65:509-518. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2064        [ Links ]

68. Blumstein DT, Fernandéz-Juricic E. A primer of conservation behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 2010.         [ Links ]

69. Griffin AS, Evans CS, Blumstein DT. Learning specificity in acquired predator recognition. Anim Behav. 2001;62:577-589. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1781        [ Links ]

70. Price-Rees SJ, Webb JK, Shine R. Reducing the impact of a toxic invader by inducing taste aversion in an imperilled native reptile predator. Anim Conserv. 2013;16:386-394. https://doi.org/10.1111/acv.12004        [ Links ]

71. Hill CM, Wallace GE. Crop protection and conflict mitigation: Reducing the costs of living alongside non-human primates. Biodivers Conserv. 2012;21:2569-2587. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-012-0318-y        [ Links ]

72. Belton LE, Ball N, Waterman JM, Bateman PW. Do Cape ground squirrels (Xerus inauris) discriminate between olfactory cues in the faeces of predators versus non-predators? Afr Zool. 2007;42:135-138. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2007.11407388        [ Links ]

73. Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev. 2007;82:291-318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x        [ Links ]

74. Downs CT, Awuah A, Jordaan M, Magagula L, Mkhize T, Paine C, et al.Too hot to sleep? Sleep behaviour and surface body temperature of Wahlberg's epauletted fruit bat. PLoS ONE. 2015;10, e0119419, 14 pages. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0119419        [ Links ]

75. Jackson TP, Mosojane S, Ferreira SM, Van Aarde RJ. Solutions for elephant Loxodonta africana crop raiding in northern Botswana: Moving away from symptomatic approaches. Oryx. 2008;42:83-91. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0030605308001117        [ Links ]

76. Henzi SP, Brown LR, Barrett L, Marais AJ. Troop size, habitat use, and diet of chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus) in commercial pine plantations: Implications for management. Int J Primatol. 2011;32:1020-1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-011-9519-6        [ Links ]

77. Widdows CD, Downs CT. A genet drive-through: are large spotted genets using urban areas for 'fast food'? A dietary analysis. Urban Ecosyst. 2015;18:907-920. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11252-015-0438-8        [ Links ]

78. Tadesse SA, Kotler BP. Impact of tourism on Nubian ibex (Capra nubiana) revealed through assessment of behavioral indicators. Behav Ecol. 2012;23:1257-1262. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars110        [ Links ]

79. Nowak K, Le Roux A, Richards SA, Scheijen CP, Hill RA. Human observers impact habituated Samango monkeys' perceived landscape of fear. Behav Ecol. 2014;25:1199-1204. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru110        [ Links ]

80. Van Wilgen BW, Boshoff N, Smit IPJ, Solano-Fernandez S, Van der Walt L. A bibliometric analysis to illustrate the role of an embedded research capability In South African National Parks. Scientometrics. 2016;107:185-212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11192-016-1879-4        [ Links ]

 

 

Correspondence:
Aliza le Roux
Email: lerouxa3@ufs.ac.za

Received: 18 Sep. 2017
Revised: 14 Dec. 2017
Accepted: 17 Jan. 2018
Published: 30 May 2018

 

 

FUNDING: None

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons