SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.113 número1-2Analysis of Ozone (O3) and Erythemal UV (EUV) measured by TOMS in the equatorial African beltHenry Selby Hele-Shaw LLD, DSc, EngD, FRS, WhSch (1854-1941): engineer, inventor and educationist índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • En proceso de indezaciónCitado por Google
  • En proceso de indezaciónSimilares en Google

Compartir


South African Journal of Science

versión On-line ISSN 1996-7489
versión impresa ISSN 0038-2353

S. Afr. j. sci. vol.113 no.1-2 Pretoria ene./feb. 2017

http://dx.doi.org/10.17159/sajs.2017/20160112 

RESEARCH ARTICLE

 

On the mental toughness of self-aware athletes: Evidence from competitive tennis players

 

 

Richard G. CowdenI, II

I'Institute of Psychological Wellbeing, North-West University, Potchefstroom, South Africa
IIDiscipline of Psychology, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Durban, South Africa

Correspondence

 

 


ABSTRACT

This study examined the relationship between mental toughness (MT) and self-awareness in a sample of 175 male and 158 female South African tennis athletes (mean age = 29.09 years, s.d. = 14.00). The participants completed the Sport Mental Toughness Questionnaire and the Self-Reflection and Insight Scale to assess MT (confidence, constancy, control) and self-awareness (self-reflection and self-insight) dimensions, respectively. Linear regression indicated that self-insight (β=0.49), but not self-reflection (β=0.02), predicted global MT. Multivariate regression analyses were significant for self-reflection (ηp2=0.11) and self-insight (ηp2=0.24). Self-reflection predicted confidence and constancy (ηp2=0.05 and 0.06, respectively), whereas self-insight predicted all three MT subcomponents (ηp2=0.12 to 0.14). The findings extend prior qualitative research evidence supporting the relevance of self-awareness to the MT of competitive tennis athletes, with self-reflection and insight forming prospective routes through which athletes' MT may be developed.
SIGNIFICANCE:
Self-awareness attributes were predictive of higher levels of mental toughness among competitive tennis players.
Dimensions of self-awareness may offer routes for developing athletes' mental toughness.

Keywords: self-reflection; self-insight; athletes; sport; South Africa


 

 

Introduction

Mental toughness (MT) is widely recognised as a fundamental attribute for attaining success in sport.1 Mentally tougher athletes maintain performance levels during adversity; perceive pressure as a challenge and a catalyst for prospering; and maintain emotional, cognitive and behavioural control despite situational stressors.2 Considering the appeal that these cognitive and behavioural signatures have to athletes, MT has become a prominent research area in the sport performance literature.3

Scholars' primary interest in MT is based on the capacity to acquire MT attributes through sport and non-sport developmental influences and experiences4, as well as through psychological interventions5. However, determining the MT dimensions that may be taught and the most effective approaches to develop them requires resolutions to the current conceptual and operational disparities that exist. Some researchers contend that MT is a narrow personality trait that is situationally stable5,6, whereas others suggest MT is state-specific and may fluctuate depending on the situation7,8. In addition to MT manifestation distinctions, these conceptualisations differ in the extent to which MT may be developed. However, in support of the mutual inclusivity of these perspectives, Gucciardi et al.3 reported that a combination of intraindividual (i.e. within person) and interindividual (i.e. between person) differences may be attributed to the variability of MT. Accordingly, an athlete may display enduring patterns of MT across similar situations, but varied levels of MT across dissimilar situations.

Although the multidimensionality of MT has generally been supported9, the type and quantity of constituents comprising MT remains unclear10. In addition to dimensional discrepancies between sport types,11 within-sport MT differences have been found. For instance, Coulter et al.12 reported that risk-taking is an integral MT component in soccer, whereas Thelwell et al.13 indicated that MT in a soccer player involved affecting one's opponents. The characterisation of MT variations are reflected in the range of instruments that often diverge in the types of MT that are measured. To illustrate, affective intelligence is included as a subfactor on the Cricket Mental Toughness Inventory14, but is not contained within the Australian Football Mental Toughness Inventory15.

Although unequivocally determining the components that constitute MT is necessary, there are several components that are repeatedly referred to in the literature.16 These components include confidence or self-belief; emotional and cognitive control; accepting, persevering and thriving through challenges; and commitment and determination.2,17 Accordingly, MT refers to a collection of personal resources (inherent and developed) associated with athletes' pursuit of optimal athletic performance levels, irrespective of positive and negative situational demands.18,19

In the extant literature, considerable attention has been devoted towards examining the characteristics associated with MT. Commonly identified correlates of MT include effective coping, the use of self-talk, relaxation strategies and mental imagery.20-22 Mentally tougher athletes have greater flow experiences (concentration, autotelism)23, perceive stressors as less intense24, and utilise performance- and mastery-approach achievement goals25. Collectively, MT is related to a number of positive psychological characteristics. However, self-awareness, also referred to as psychological self-mindedness, is one concept that has received limited quantitative MT research attention. Self-awareness represents the capacity to attend to, recognise and examine one's thoughts, physiological sensations, emotions and behavioural reactions, either as they occur or retrospectively.26,27

Although the self-awareness process is multifaceted and associated with an array of corollaries and self-directed attention areas26, common conceptualisations encompass two primary components: engagement in self-reflection and the attainment of self-insight28-30. Self-reflection involves emotional, cognitive and behavioural self-introspection, whereas self-insight refers to clarifying and obtaining a deeper understanding of such experiences.29

Even though self-reflective activities may not automatically result in self-insight31, self-awareness represents an important process for identifying and replacing maladaptive responses as well as establishing progress towards achieving positive psychobehavioural changes28,32.

In sport, awareness of one's emotions has been linked to superior performance.33 In particular, maintaining peak performance levels is at least partly dependent on the ability to recognise negative emotions and cognitions and effectively control or avoid the detrimental effects of such experiences.34 With research supporting the emotional and cognitive control of mentally tough athletes35, along with the understanding that MT is associated with positive performance outcomes3, self-awareness attributes may be relevant to athletes' MT.

Recent qualitative research has posited the relevance of several forms of self-awareness (e.g. emotional and cognitive) in relation to MT. Bull et al.25, for instance, qualitatively established thinking clearly (awareness, focus and control of thoughts) as an essential component of MT in elite cricket. Slack et al.36 extended this finding to denote cognitive awareness of own emotions as indicative of mentally tough English Premier League football referees. There is also evidence to suggest that self-awareness promotes or facilitates heightened levels of MT37 - a finding that supports early heuristic MT perspectives38.

Taken together, these findings provide preliminary support for the applicability of self-awareness characteristics to the MT of athletes. However, prior MT studies have not specified what embodies self-awareness, and, despite recent qualitative findings, there is a dearth of knowledge about the role of emotional, cognitive and behavioural self-awareness in relation to MT. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to explore the relationships between MT and self-awareness components (i.e. self-reflection and self-insight) in competitive tennis players. It was hypothesised that MT and each of its subcomponents would be significantly predicted by both (1) self-reflection and (2) self-insight.

 

Method

Participants

The participants were 175 male (mean(s.d.) age = 31.99(15.64) years) and 158 female (mean(s.d.) age = 25.89(11.12) years) tennis players competing at various levels: county club (n=58), local county tournament (n=21), university league (n=147), national tournament (n=76) and international tournament (n=31). The athletes had played tennis for a minimum of 5 years (mean(s.d.) of 17.13(12.27) years) and had engaged in tennis competition within 2 weeks prior to their participation in the study.

Materials

Mental toughness

The Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ)17, which comprises 14 Likert-type items rated from 1 ('not at all true') to 4 ('very true'), was used to ascertain MT. As a multidimensional measure of MT developed from the most common components of MT identified in the literature,39 the SMTQ measures control, confidence and constancy. There are four control items (e.g. 'I am overcome by self-doubt'), six confidence items (e.g. 'I interpret potential threats as positive opportunities') and four constancy items (e.g. 'I take responsibility for setting myself challenging targets'). The subscales may be combined for a global measure of MT The selection of the SMTQ was based on the demonstrated validity (i.e. factorial, divergent, discriminative) and reliability of the instrument reported in the initial validation study.17 Subsequent studies have supported the convergent validity40,41 and internal consistency of global MT.42,43 In this study, Cronbach's alpha for global MT was 0.75.

With alpha inclined to underestimate internal consistency when fewer than 10 items are included on a scale, mean inter-item correlations are important for assessing scalar homogeneity.44 According to Briggs and Cheek45, mean inter-item correlation values with a range of 0.2-0.4 indicate appropriate item homogeneity. The internal consistency estimates (and mean inter-item correlations) for confidence, constancy and control were 0.64 (0.23), 0.56 (0.25) and 0.66 (0.33), respectively.

Self-awareness

The Self-Reflection and Insight Scale (SRIS)29 was used to assess self-awareness. The SRIS comprises 20 Likert-type items (1 = 'strongly disagree', 6 = 'strongly agree') on two subscales: self-reflection (12 items) and self-insight (8 items). Self-reflection measures one's need for and engagement in self-evaluation (e.g. 'I frequently examine my feelings') and self-insight assesses the lucidity of one's thought, emotional and behavioural understanding (e.g. 'I usually know why I feel the way I do'). The SRIS has received construct, convergent and cross-cultural validity support29,46 and both subscales have evidenced acceptable internal consistency and test-retest reliability29,47-48. Internal consistency for self-reflection in this study was 0.90, and alpha (and the mean inter-item correlation) for self-insight was 0.78 (0.30).

Procedure

Permission letters were obtained from relevant tennis organisations in order to acquire institutional ethical approval to conduct the study. Full ethical approval was subsequently granted by the University of KwaZulu-Natal Humanities and Social Sciences Research Ethics Committee (HSS/0740/013D). Suitable tennis tournaments were identified, and the organisers were approached in order to request permission to access the competitive tennis players. Various tennis tournament and club venues across South Africa were attended and the self-administered questionnaires were distributed in groups of approximately 5 to 10 athletes at a time, according to the players' availability. Informed consent was obtained prior to the players' participation, and all relevant Declaration of Helsinki principles were adhered to. A quiet and comfortable venue was established at each location for completion of the questionnaire. The inventories required approximately 15 to 20 minutes to complete; each player completed the SMTQ followed by the SRIS. The participants were requested to consider the extent to which each item applied to them, generally, in relation to their participation in competitive tennis.

Data analyses

Box-plot assessment revealed a small number of gross outliers on the global MT scale and subscales. These individual case values were removed before computing the analyses (see Table 1). Normality estimates (i.e. skewness and kurtosis) were within acceptable limits (i.e. ±2)49 for proceeding with parametric computations. Along with these estimates, the descriptive statistics for each variable and bivariate relationships are reported in Table 1. After satisfying the hypothesis testing assumptions associated with conducting parametric regression analyses (e.g. normality, homoscedasticity), multiple linear regression and multivariate regression were used to determine whether self-reflection and self-insight predicted global MT and each of the MT components, respectively. For significant multivariate analyses, a Bonferroni adjustment was applied to follow-up univariate p-values to preserve familywise alpha. An alpha value of 0.05 was used for each statistical test.

 

Results

Bivariate analyses

According to Cohen's50 effect size standards, the correlations between global MT and self-reflection (r2=0.02) and self-insight (r2=0.25) were small and large, respectively (see Table 1). With the exception of control, which was not significantly associated with self-reflection (r2=0.00), the relationships between the MT subcomponents and self-reflection and insight were medium in effect size (r2 =0.06 to 0.15).

Univariate and multivariate analyses

The multiple linear regression results indicated that self-insight (3=0.49, p<0.001, 95% CI [0.41, 0.57]), but not self-reflection (0=0.02, p=0.652, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.13]), significantly predicted global MT: F(2, 327)=54.38, p<0.001, r2=0.25, 95% CI [0.17, 0.33].

Multivariate regression revealed self-reflection, F(3, 318)=12.87, p<0.001, Wilk's // = 0.89, ηp2=0.11, 95% CI [0.05, 0.17], and self-insight, F(3, 318)=33.43, p<0.001, Wilk's /1 = 0.76, ηp2=0.24, 95% CI [0.15, 0.31], significantly predicted one or more subcomponent of MT. Specifically, self-reflection predicted confidence, F(1, 320)=16.85, p<0.001, ηp2=0.05, 95% CI [0.01, 0.10], and constancy, F(1, 320)=21.72, p<0.001, ηp2=0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.12], but not control, F(1, 320)=1.47, p=0.678, ηp2;=0.01, 95% CI [0.00, 0.03]. Self-insight was a significant predictor of confidence, F(1, 320)=44.01, p<0.001, ηp2=0.12, 95% CI [0.06, 0.19], constancy, F(1, 320)=51.46, p<0.001, ^=0.14, 95% CI [0.08, 0.21], and control, F(1, 320)=51.37, p<0.001, ηp2=0.14, 95% CI [0.08, 0.21].

 

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine the relationships between MT and self-awareness dimensions among competitive tennis players. The results provided partial support for the hypotheses, as global MT was predicted by self-insight (i.e. clarity of thought, emotional and behavioural understanding) and not by self-reflection (i.e. need for and enagement in psychobehavioural self-evaluation). In addition, confidence and constancy were each predicted by self-reflection and self-insight, although control was only predicted by self-insight. Collectively, the results support superior thought, emotional and behavioural awareness among mentally tougher tennis athletes, which is consistent with qualitative research denoting the relevance of various forms of self-awareness to MT.25,36

The finding that self-insight was the single significant predictor of global MT suggests the phase is particularly important to athletes' MT. However, given that attaining insight requires introspection and evaluation of the self,31 self-reflection is a necessary part of the self-awareness process. Considering the markedly larger effect size between MT and self-insight, as compared to self-reflection, mentally tougher athletes appear to be better at progressing from self-reflective activities to achieve higher levels of self-insight. With prior studies reporting that self-awareness promotes the development of MT,37 attaining maximal MT benefits might require athletes to engage in and proceed beyond mere self-introspection toward generating a profounder level of psychobehavioural clarity and understanding.

Comparable to the global MT outcomes, the effect sizes in relation to confidence, constancy and control were larger for self-insight. Therefore, in contrast to tennis players who are largely self-reflective, those who are more self-insightful are substantially more likely to possess higher levels of confidence, constancy and control. Accordingly, the advantage of self-awareness to athletes' MT appears to depend more strongly on obtaining self-insight than on engaging in self-reflection alone.

The findings in this study indicate self-awareness is an important determinant of an athlete's confidence and belief.51 In fact, Beaumont et al.52 found that developing athletes' awareness of their thoughts and feelings is an effective strategy for fostering their confidence. Referring to an athlete's perceived ability to succeed53, confidence is enhanced by reflecting on events related to the self, particularly achievements and positive experiences4. This would suggest that mentally tougher athletes maintain or develop confidence levels by emphasising the strengths and encouraging features of events and their responses. Further intimating the role of reflection and insight, Jones et al.2 found that mentally tough athletes' belief is cultivated from recognising the process involved in reaching their level of achievement. The optimistic perspective associated with mentally tough athletes54 could also account for their persistent sense of confidence12 despite the psychobehavioural assessment and clarity that may accompany their weaknesses or negative outcomes. Whether mentally tougher athletes display a greater tendency to focus on the positive features of various situations, outcomes or consequences is an area that requires further examination.

Tennis players with greater self-reflective and insightful tendencies also exhibited higher levels of constancy. This finding suggests that athletes who think about and distinctly understand their thoughts, emotions and behaviours are more likely to remain determined and committed. Given that mentally tough athletes optimistically reflect on circumstances and events,12 positive perceptions about the causes of and explanations for their psychobehavioural responses in such moments may enable them to remain resolute in their efforts and goal pursuits. The positive appraisal orientation that characterises MT is supported by Kaiseler et al.'s24 study, which found that athletes with higher levels of MT rated stressors as less intense. Furthermore, Mahoney et al.55 associate MT with striving (i.e. sustained, consistent effort) and thriving (i.e. learning and growth). Through mentally tougher athletes' heightened self-awareness, these qualities likely contribute to appraising their negative responses as opportunities to overcome their weaknesses and improve (i.e. thrive), thereby promoting commitment and dedication (i.e. striving).

The finding that self-insight predicted control supports self-awareness as a fundamental prerequisite to athletes' aptitude to maintain control.56 Specifically, athletes' emotional and cognitive control could be promoted by clearly and extensively understanding their emotional, cognitive and behavioural experiences. Their superior self-insight might allow mentally tougher athletes to select and effectively use psychological skills20 to control their thoughts and emotions when they arise during competition. This supposition is supported by Mahoney et al.'s55 notion that mentally tougher athletes' awareness of their emotional experiences enables them to choose an appropriate coping strategy to maintain desired performance levels. Considering athletes utilise a range of cognitive control strategies (e.g. word cues) during training and competitive performance contexts,57 self-insight might be critical to athletes' employment of situationally specific psychological strategies to sustain athletic performance.

 

Practical suggestions

Given the role of self-awareness in the growth of MT37, coupled with the capacity to develop MT through specific training programmes8, self-awareness offers a potential target area for MT interventions. Recent findings have indicated reflective practices may contribute to an athlete's awareness and athletic development.58 The propensity to self-reflect may be enhanced through the use of a logbook52 - an exercise that also increases athletes' awareness of negative self-talk, the situations in which it occurs, and the outcomes associated with it59. A similar logbook, diary, or written prompt method could be used to facilitate players' self-reflection and enhance their understanding of the cognitive and emotional experiences that occur when training or competing. Through the process of identifying the antecedents and consequences of their particularly negative responses, more adaptive future responses that reflect MT (e.g. emotional control) may be engendered.

Using this type of framework, tennis players may engage in postcompetition assessment of the moments in which they experienced positive and negative thoughts (e.g. 'I'm going to lose my serve') and behaviours (e.g. racquet tossing, self-degrading comments). Explanations for these thoughts (e.g. focusing on losing a service game when break point down) or behaviours (e.g. racquet tossing following a loss of serve), along with the outcome following such thoughts or behaviours (e.g. periodic performance slumps), could subsequently be examined. With the support of sport psychology professionals or coaches, this in-depth evaluation and identification of what, when and why thoughts, emotions and behaviours occurred may be used to replace maladaptive responses that emerged previously.

The process through which maladaptive responses are changed could be facilitated in several ways, particularly cognitively. Resulting from their heightened self-awareness, athletes' deleterious responses may be altered through more favourably reappraising the situations in which the responses were triggered.60 Alternatively, athletes could develop attentional deployment skills61 in order to focus on the positive features of situations (e.g. one's own physical and technical strengths). Another avenue to changing athletes' emotional and behavioural responses involves cognitive reframing - an approach that encourages the contestation and replacement of debilitative, negative ideas and beliefs with positive, facilitative thought processes.62 Research has found that reframing is beneficial to generating facilitative perceptions about the influence of competitive anxiety and physiological arousal on athletes' performance.63 Taken together, these cognitive strategies represent some of the mechanisms through which athletes' could control their thoughts and emotions, develop confidence and maintain commitment levels52 following their self-reflective activities and attainment of insight.

 

Limitations and future research directions

Selected methodological limitations should be considered alongside the contributions of this study. While a purposeful decision was made to examine MT within a specific group of athletes (i.e. competitive tennis players), the generalisability of the findings to other sports is questionable. In addition, the non-experimental approach restricts conclusions of causality among the variables included in the study. This could be addressed through experimental and longitudinal MT and athletic performance level studies that target or manipulate self-awareness. Another drawback is that the data were sourced solely from the athletes, and the inclusion of additional data sources (e.g. coaches) would have provided an opportunity to cross-verify the participants' self-reports. The measurement of participants' average MT and self-awareness in tennis is another limitation, with the cross-situational applicability and variability of self-awareness, MT and the relationships between the two constructs indeterminable. Future research might explore athletes' self-awareness processes and MT responses following different types of stressors (e.g. inclement weather conditions) and competitive phases (e.g. ahead versus behind). The findings should also be interpreted in conjunction with the criticisms of the SMTQ, such as its brevity, partial conceptual coverage of MT16 and logical validity concerns42. Although the SMTQ has been validated and has received psychometric support, additional validation studies may be required to refine the measure.

 

Conclusion

The findings in this study support the positive association between MT and self-awareness in competitive tennis players. Most notably, the strongest predictor of MT and its subcomponents was self-insight. Notwithstanding the necessity of self-reflection in the process toward obtaining insight, the latter appears to be particularly important when considering MT and its development among athletes. Research identifying the contextual demands and situation-based use of self-awareness among mentally tough athletes is warranted, along with whether self-reflection and insight may be used to develop MT through interventions.

 

References

1. Connaughton D, Hanton S, Jones G, Wadey R. Mental toughness research: Key issues in this area. Int J Sport Psychol. 2008;39:192-204.         [ Links ]

2. Jones G, Hanton S, Connaughton D. A framework of mental toughness in the world's best performers. Sport Psychol. 2007;21:243-264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.21.2.243        [ Links ]

3. Gucciardi D, Hanton S, Gordon S, Mallett C, Temby P. The concept of mental toughness: Tests of dimensionality, nomological network, and traitness. J Pers. 2015;83:26-14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12079        [ Links ]

4. Connaughton D, Hanton S, Jones G. The development and maintenance of mental toughness in the world's best performers. Sport Psychol. 2010;24:168-193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.24.2.168        [ Links ]

5. Bell J, Hardy L, Beattie S. Enhancing mental toughness and performance under pressure in elite young cricketers: A 2-year longitudinal intervention. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2013;2:281-297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033129        [ Links ]

6. St Clair-Thompson H, Bugler M, Robinson J, Clough P McGeown S, Perry J. Mental toughness in education: Exploring relationships with attainment, attendance, behaviour and peer relationships. Educ Psychol Int J Exp Educ Psychol. 2015;35:886-907. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.895294        [ Links ]

7. Harmison R. A social-cognitive framework for understanding and developing mental toughness in sport. In: Gucciardi D, Gordon S, editors. Mental toughness in sport: Developments in research and theory. New York:Routledge; 2011. p. 47-68.         [ Links ]

8. Slack L, Maynard I, Butt J, Olusaga P. An evaluation of a mental toughness education and training program for early-career English Football League referees. Sport Psychol. 2015;29:237-257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.2014-0015        [ Links ]

9. Hardy L, Bell J, Beattie S. A neuropsychological model of mentally tough behavior. J Pers. 2014;82:69-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12034        [ Links ]

10. Gucciardi D, Jackson B, Hanton S, Reid M. Motivational correlates of mentally tough behaviours in tennis. J Sci Med Sport. 2015;18:67-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2013.11.009        [ Links ]

11. Crust L. A review and conceptual re-examination of mental toughness: Implications for future researchers. Pers Indiv Differ. 2008;45:576-583. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.005        [ Links ]

12. Coulter T, Mallett C, Gucciardi D. Understanding mental toughness in Australian soccer: Perceptions of players, parents, and coaches. J Sports Sci. 2010;28:699-716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640411003734085        [ Links ]

13. Thelwell R, Weston N, Greenlees I. Defining and understanding mental toughness within soccer. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2005;17:326-332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200500313636        [ Links ]

14. Gucciardi D, Gordon S. Development and preliminary validation of the Cricket Mental Toughness Inventory (CMTI). J Sports Sci. 2009;27:1293-1310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640410903242306        [ Links ]

15. Gucciardi D, Gordon S, Dimmock J. Development and preliminary validation of a mental toughness inventory for Australian football. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2009;10:201-209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.07.011        [ Links ]

16. Gucciardi D, Mallett C, Hanrahan S, Gordon S. Measuring mental toughness in sport: Current status and future directions. In: Gucciardi D, Gordon S, editors. Mental toughness in sport: Developments in theory and research. New York: Routledge; 2011. p. 108-132.         [ Links ]

17. Sheard M, Golby J, Van Wersch A. Progress toward construct validation of the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2009;25:186-193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.25.3.186        [ Links ]

18. Cook C, Crust L, Littlewood M, Nesti M, Allen-Collinson J. 'What it takes': Perceptions of mental toughness and its development in an English Premier League Soccer Academy. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health. 2014;6:329-347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/2159676X.2013.857708        [ Links ]

19. Gucciardi D, Jackson B, Hodge K, Anthony A, Brooke L. Implicit theories of mental toughness: Relations with cognitive, motivational, and behavioral correlates. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2015;4:100-112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/spy0000024        [ Links ]

20. Crust L, Azadi K. Mental toughness and athletes' use of psychological strategies. Eur J Sport Sci. 2010;10:43-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461390903049972        [ Links ]

21. Mattie P, Munroe-Chandler K. Examining the relationship between mental toughness and imagery use. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2012;24:144-156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200.2011.605422        [ Links ]

22. Nicholls A, Levy A, Polman R, Crust L. Mental toughness, coping self- efficacy, and coping effectiveness among athletes. Int J Sport Psychol. 2011;42:513-524.         [ Links ]

23. Crust L, Swann C. The relationship between mental toughness and dispositional flow. Eur J Sport Sci. 2013;13:215-220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2011.635698        [ Links ]

24. Kaiseler M, Polman R, Nicholls A. Mental toughness, stress, stress appraisal, coping and coping effectiveness in sport. Pers Indiv Differ. 2009;47:728-733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.012        [ Links ]

25. Bull S, Shambrook C, James W, Brooks J. Towards an understanding of mental toughness in elite English cricketers. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2005;17:209-227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200591010085        [ Links ]

26. Morin A. Self-awareness part 1: Definition, measures, effects, functions, and antecedents. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2011;5:807-823. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00387.x        [ Links ]

27. Pieterse A, Lee M, Ritmeester A, Collins N. Towards a model of self-awareness development for counselling and psychotherapy training. Couns Psychol Q. 2013;26:190-207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09515070.2013.793451        [ Links ]

28. Grant A. Rethinking psychological mindedness: Metacognition, self-reflection and insight. Behav Change. 2001;18:8-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/bech.18.1.8        [ Links ]

29. Grant A, Franklin J, Langford P. The self-reflection and insight scale: A new measure of private self-consciousness. Soc Behav Pers. 2002;30:821-836. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2002.30.8.821        [ Links ]

30. Xu X. Self-reflection, insight, and individual differences in various language tasks. Psychol Rec. 2011;61:41-58.         [ Links ]

31. Hixon J, Swann W. When does introspection bear fruit? Self-reflection, self- insight, and interpersonal choices. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:35-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.L35        [ Links ]

32. Carver C, Scheier M. On the self-regulation of behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174794        [ Links ]

33. Zizzi S, Deaner H, Hirschhorn D. The relationship between emotional intelligence and performance among college baseball players. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2003;15:262-269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200305390        [ Links ]

34. Krane V Williams J. Psychological characteristics of peak performance. In: Williams J, editor. Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak performance. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2006. p. 207-227.         [ Links ]

35. Gucciardi D, Gordon S, Dimmock J. Advancing mental toughness research and theory using personal construct psychology. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2009;2:54-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17509840802705938        [ Links ]

36. Slack L, Butt J, Maynard I, Olusoga P. Understanding mental toughness in elite football officiating: Perceptions of English Premier League referees. Sport Exerc Psychol Rev. 2014;10:4-24.         [ Links ]

37. Gucciardi D, Gordon S, Dimmock J. Evaluation of a mental toughness training program for youth-aged Australian footballers: I. A qualitative analysis. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2009;21:324-339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200903026074        [ Links ]

38. Loehr J. The new mental toughness training for sport. New York: Penguin; 1995.         [ Links ]

39. Sheard M. Mental toughness: The mindset behind sporting achievement. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge; 2013.         [ Links ]

40. Chen M, Cheesman D. Mental toughness of mixed martial arts athletes at different levels of competition. Percept Mot Skills. 2013;116:905-917. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/29.30.PMS.116.3.905-917        [ Links ]

41. Meggs J, Ditzfeld C, Golby J. Self-concept organisation and mental toughness in sport. J Sports Sci. 2014;32:101-109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2013.812230        [ Links ]

42. Crust L, Swann C. Comparing two measures of mental toughness. Pers Indiv Differ. 2011;50:217-221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.09.032        [ Links ]

43. Petrie T, Deiters J, Harmison R. Mental toughness, social support, and athletic identity: Moderators of the life stress-injury relationship in collegiate football players. Sport Exerc Perform Psychol. 2014;3:13-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032698        [ Links ]

44. Pearson R. Statistical persuasion: How to collect, analyze, and present data accurately, honestly, and persuasively. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781452230122        [ Links ]

45. Briggs S, Cheek J. The role of factor analysis in the development and evaluation of personality scales. J Pers. 1986;54:106-148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1986.tb00391.x        [ Links ]

46. DaSilveira A, DeCastro T, Gomes W. Escala de Autorreflexäo e Insight: Nova medida de autoconsciência adaptada e validada para adultos Brasileiros [Self- Reflection and Insight Scale: New self-consciousness measure adapted and validated to Brazilian adults]. Psico-PUCRS. 2012;43:155-162. Portugese.         [ Links ]

47. DaSilveira A, DeSouza M, Gomes W. Self-consciousness concept and assessment in self-report measures. Front Psychol. 2015;6, Art. #930, 11 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00930        [ Links ]

48. Roberts L, Heritage B, Gasson N. The measurement of psychological literacy: A first approximation. Front Psychol. 2015;6, Art. #105, 12 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00105        [ Links ]

49. Chou C, Bentler P Estimates and tests in structural equation modeling. In: Hoyle R, editor. Structural equation modelling: Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1995. p. 37-55.         [ Links ]

50. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155        [ Links ]

51. Hays K, Maynard I, Thomas O, Bawden M. Sources and types of confidence identified by world class sport performers. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007;19:434-156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200701599173        [ Links ]

52. Beaumont C, Maynard I, Butt J. Effective ways to develop and maintain robust sport-confidence: Strategies advocated by sport psychology consultants. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2015;27:301-318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413200.2014.996302        [ Links ]

53. Vealey R. Understanding and enhancing self-confidence in athletes. In: Singer R, Hausenblas H, Janelle C, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley; 2001. p. 550-565.         [ Links ]

54. Guillén F, Laborde S. Higher-order structure of mental toughness and the analysis of latent mean differences between athletes from 34 disciplines and non-athletes. Pers Indiv Differ. 2014;60:30-35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.019        [ Links ]

55. Mahoney J, Gucciardi D, Ntoumanis N, Mallett C. The motivational antecedents of the development of mental toughness: A self-determination theory perspective. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2014;7:184-197. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2014.925951        [ Links ]

56. Brunelle J, Janelle C, Tennant L. Controlling competitive anger among male soccer players. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1999;11:283-297. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10413209908404205        [ Links ]

57. Toner J, Montero B, Moran A. Considering the role of cognitive control in expert performance. Phenomenol Cogn Sci. 2014;14:1127-1144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9407-6        [ Links ]

58. Faull A, Cropley B. Reflective learning in sport: A case study of a senior level triathlete. Refl Pract. 2009;10:325-339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14623940903034655        [ Links ]

59. Hardy J, Roberts R, Hardy L. Awareness and motivation to change negative self-talk. Sport Psychol. 2009;23:435-450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.23.4.435        [ Links ]

60. Lane A, Beedie C, Jones M, Uphill M, Devonport T. The BASES Expert Statement on emotion regulation in sport. J Sports Sci. 2012;30:1189-1195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2012.693621        [ Links ]

61. Webb T, Miles E, Sheeran P. Dealing with feeling: A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation. Psychol Bull. 2012;138:775-808. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027600        [ Links ]

62. Mennis D, Ellard K, Fresco D, Gross J. United we stand: Emphasizing commonalities across cognitive-behavioral therapies. Behav Ther. 2013;44:234-248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2013.02.004        [ Links ]

63. Moore L, Vine S, Wilson M, Freeman P. Reappraising threat: How to optimize performance under pressure. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2015;37:339-343. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2014-0186        [ Links ]

 

 

Correspondence:
Richard Cowden
richardgregorycowden@gmail.com

Received: 08 Apr. 2016
Revised: 15 July 2016
Accepted: 07 Sep. 2016

 

 

FUNDING: None

Creative Commons License Todo el contenido de esta revista, excepto dónde está identificado, está bajo una Licencia Creative Commons